Developmental Psychology
The journal provides a forum for sharing information and experience both in the field of developmental research and practices that are related to the psychological diagnosis and support for development.
See issuesThe journal provides a forum for sharing information and experience both in the field of developmental research and practices that are related to the psychological diagnosis and support for development.
See issuesAffiliation: Polskie Stowarzyszenie Psychologii Rozwoju Człowieka
Periodicity: QuarterlyYear of foundation: 1993
Article languages english, polish
Status: active
Scientific domain: Family studies, Psychology, Social sciences
Journal type: Scientific
ISSN: 1895-6297
eISSN: 2084-3879
UIC ID: 200179
DOI: 10.4467/20843879PR
MNiSW points: 40
Wersja papierowa: tak
Wydawane od: 1993
Journal licence: CC BY, open access
Publication date: 21.03.2025
Editor of the issue: Dorota Czyżowska
Associate editors: Dorota Czyżowska, Joanna Kossewska, Małgorzata Stępień-Nycz
Editor-in-Chief: Maria Kielar-Turska
This publication has been supported by The Polish Association of Human Development Psychology.
Adam Niemczyński
Developmental Psychology, Vol. 28, Issue 4, 2023, pp. 9-10
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.23.015.20853Elżbieta Dryll
Developmental Psychology, Vol. 28, Issue 4, 2023, pp. 11-20
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.23.016.20854Marian Olejnik
Developmental Psychology, Vol. 28, Issue 4, 2023, pp. 21-26
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.23.017.20855Adam Niemczyński
Developmental Psychology, Vol. 28, Issue 4, 2023, pp. 27-44
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.23.018.20856Szymon Chrząstowski, Grażyna Kmita, Anna Strzałkowska
Developmental Psychology, Vol. 28, Issue 4, 2023, pp. 45-57
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.23.019.20857Grażyna Mendecka
Developmental Psychology, Vol. 28, Issue 4, 2023, pp. 61-78
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.23.020.20858Łukasz Nikel , Maria Kędzierska
Developmental Psychology, Vol. 28, Issue 4, 2023, pp. 79-93
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.23.021.20859Issue Editor: Joanna Kossewska
Editor-in-Chief: Maria Kielar-Turska
This publication has been supported by The Polish Association of Human Development Psychology.
Anna Hryniewicka
Developmental Psychology, Volume 28 Issue 3, 2023, pp. 9-28
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.23.008.19698This article is dedicated to Maria Grzegorzewska – an outstanding creator of the Polish school of special pedagogy, initiator, founder, and long-time director of the oldest university in Poland training special educators, and one of the most outstanding women in psychology of the 20th century, honored by the Sejm of the Republic of Poland on the 100th anniversary of the founding of the Academy of Special Pedagogy with the honorable title of Patroness of the year 2022. It presents the most important facts about her life and activities, the origins of her scientific interests, the sources of her creativity that determined how to coped with the challenges related to realizingher vocation to serve people with disabilities, her contribution to the development of various areas of culture and science, and the extraordinary power of influence on colleagues and students, thanks to which the knowledge system she created remains valid today and can set the direction for the further development of pedagogy and related disciplines.
Ewa Zasępa, Izabela Zębrowska
Developmental Psychology, Volume 28 Issue 3, 2023, pp. 29-40
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.23.009.19699The article presents a review of research on the prevalence of emotional and behavioral problems in children and adolescents raised in foster families. Epidemiological data indicate that these problems are significantly more in this group compared to children raised in biological families. Analysis of the results of the study indicates that children and adolescents from foster families are relatively more often observed to have behavioral problems than emotional problems. The article also presents factors that may increase the risk of these problems. They are divided into internal and external, environmental factors. The most common problems are polyetiological in nature. Also, the support given to the child and family should take into account multiple spheres and ways of influence.
Monika Olga Jańczak, Marta Ornoch
Developmental Psychology, Volume 28 Issue 3, 2023, pp. 43-58
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.23.010.19700In this study, we aimed to investigate the pathomechanism of eating disorders (ED) by examining mentalizing, attachment, and defense mechanisms. We conducted a study with 85 young women who completed the ECR-R, MentS, DSQ-40, and EAT-26 questionnaires. Women displaying high ED symptoms showed increased attachment anxiety and avoidance, utilized more immature defense mechanisms, and had lower levels of self-reflective mentalizing compared to the control group. There were no significant differences in interpersonal mentalizing and motivation for mentalizing. Regression analysis revealed that predictors of ED included attachment anxiety, attachment avoidance, and self-reflective mentalizing, explaining 47% of the variance in ED symptoms. Additionally, self-reflective mentalizing mediated the relationship between attachment anxiety, attachment avoidance, and the severity of ED symptoms.
Beata Nisiewicz, Bożena Gulla
Developmental Psychology, Volume 28 Issue 3, 2023, pp. 59-73
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.23.011.19701Manifestations of high sensitivity can be recognized even in the youngest children. Therefore, it is not possible to test it through activities that may disrupt its proper functioning. The research presented aims to investigate the development of this trait. There have been no longitudinal studies focusing on this trait in children, as they have not been widely researched or passed between devices that connect children with their parents. The study analyzed data from 131 children aged 8–11, including 84 tested twice, and 33 pairs of parents. The Highly Sensitive Child Scale in the Polish adaptation by Baryła-Matejczuk and Poleszak, and the Highly Sensitive Person Scale translated by Golonka and Gulla were used for measurement.
The collected data confirm the validity of distinguishing three significantly different groups among children that are under control. It was noticed that girls have a higher sensitivity level than boys, and high sensitivity characterizes them more often than boys at a statistically significant level. The similarity in sensitivity levels between children and their parents has also been confirmed, along with the statistical significance in both parents. The percentage of highly sensitive children in the study group does not align with Elaine Aron’s theory assumptions and previous research findings. Additional predictions have been partially supported by the data.
While the obtained validation results have been expanded upon, it is important to note the absence of this type of testing, and these initial findings can serve as preliminary values for further in-depth analyses in the future.
Bożena Gulla, Beata Nisiewicz
Developmental Psychology, Volume 28 Issue 3, 2023, pp. 75-88
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.23.014.20082Manifestations of high sensitivity can be recognized even in the youngest children. Therefore, it is not possible to test it through activities that may disrupt its proper functioning. The research presented aims to investigate the development of this trait. There have been no longitudinal studies focusing on this trait in children, as they have not been widely researched or passed between devices that connect children with their parents. The study analyzed data from 131 children aged 8–11, including 84 tested twice, and 33 pairs of parents. The Highly Sensitive Child Scale in the Polish adaptation by Baryła-Matejczuk and Poleszak, and the Highly Sensitive Person Scale translated by Golonka and Gulla were used for measurement.
The collected data confirm the validity of distinguishing three significantly different groups among children that are under control. It was noticed that girls have a higher sensitivity level than boys, and high sensitivity characterizes them more often than boys at a statistically significant level. The similarity in sensitivity levels between children and their parents has also been confirmed, along with the statistical significance in both parents. The percentage of highly sensitive children in the study group does not align with Elaine Aron’s theory assumptions and previous research findings. Additional predictions have been partially supported by the data.
While the obtained validation results have been expanded upon, it is important to note the absence of this type of testing, and these initial findings can serve as preliminary values for further in-depth analyses in the future.
Anna Misztela, Sabina Barszcz, Aleksandra M. Słowińska
Developmental Psychology, Volume 28 Issue 3, 2023, pp. 89-99
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.23.012.19702The main goal of this research was to recognize the psychosocial consequences of undertaking the developmental tasks in early adulthood. We wanted to focus on verification of the influency developmental tasks (undertaking them, their importance, satisfaction, and punctuality) on the sense of adulthood, and life satisfaction. The sample consisted of 343 people, 19–35 years old. The results showed that: (1) all studied aspects of developmental tasks have a positive influence on the sense of adulthood; (2) stronger sense of adulthood and (3) undertaking two developmental tasks (marriage/partnership, professional activity) result in a higher life satisfaction.
Dominik D. Strzelecki
Developmental Psychology, Volume 28 Issue 3, 2023, pp. 101-115
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.23.013.19703The challenges of simultaneously experiencing depressive disorders and autism spectrum conditions pose a significant threat to the well-being of young adult women. Previous research reports indicate that engagement in meaningful activities, specifically analysed in occupational therapy, can not only increase the effectiveness of the activities performed, but also have a positive impact on overall life satisfaction and mental health. The purpose of this study was to demonstrate the relationship between the severity of autistic traits, involvement in meaningful activities and depression in women. The following tools were used in the study: Short Autism Spectrum Quotient Test (AQ-10), Engagement in Meaningful Activities Scale (EMAS) and Beck Depression Inventory (BDI). The study involved 237 women aged 18 to 35 years (M = 21.1; SD = 1.93), who were divided into two subgroups based on AQ-10 indicators. The results revealed a linear relationship between the severity of autistic traits and involvement in meaningful activities and depressive symptoms. Additionally, a correlation was found between engagement in meaningful activities and depression. It was also identified that the intensification of autistic traits significantly differentiated the level of engagement in meaningful activities but did not differentiate the level of depression.
Publication date: 06.09.2024
Editor of the issue: Małgorzata Stępień-Nycz
Associate editors: Dorota Czyżowska, Joanna Kossewska, Małgorzata Stępień-Nycz
Editor-in-Chief: Maria Kielar-Turska
This publication has been supported by The Polish Association of Human Development Psychology.
Małgorzata Rękosiewicz
Developmental Psychology, Volume 28, Issue 2, 2023, pp. 9-23
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.23.014.19851The number of families in which at least one parent has an intellectual disability remains unknown. Little is also known about the experiences of children growing up in such families. Single studies (e.g. retrospective studies of adult children) provide knowledge about such risk factors for child and family development as parentification or social stigma. The article presents the results of a systematic review of the literature (using the PsycArticles and PsycInfo databases) on the development of children in families with at least one parent with intellectual disability. The aim of the review was to explore the empirical research conducted so far in this area. The article ends with a list of recommendations for further empirical research.
Małgorzata Rękosiewicz
Developmental Psychology, Volume 28, Issue 2, 2023, pp. 25-40
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.23.020.19913Katarzyna Tomaszek, Agnieszka Muchacka-Cymerman, Ayşe Aypay
Developmental Psychology, Volume 28, Issue 2, 2023, pp. 43-61
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.23.015.19852Background: Numerous prior studies have been tested the associations between stress, coping and burnout. In fact, all three abovementioned constructs are inevitably connected with each other according to many psychological theories. Most of them, however, focused on job-stress related context and were conducted among university students or adults. Much less frequently the specific coping techniques and its relation to burnout symptoms among high school students were analyzed.
Objective: The main purpose of the current study was to investigate the associations between coping strategies and burnout syndrome among late adolescents. Secondly we tested if student burnout is a function of gender and age. Additionally, we also tested the cross-cultural validity of student burnout scale (SSBS) by Aypay.
Methods: 761 students (35.8% boys), aged 17–19 years participated in the study.
Results: The results indicate that student burnout is associated positively with poor coping strategies and negatively with searching for support and active coping strategies. Regression models revealed that higher avoiding behaviors and helplessness, and lower active coping are significant predictors of school burnout, explaining 13% of its variance. The final model consisting of coping strategies and sociodemographic characteristics (age and gender) explained 20% of variance in the SSBS score. According to our studies girls are more prone to develop school burnout syndrome than boys, and older students are more burned out than younger ones. The findings also confirmed that the SSBS scale may be applied in the Polish context as it has good psychometric properties.
Conclusion: The findings suggest that the avoiding behaviors as well as lack of active coping may play a pivotal role in school burnout among youth.
Jerzy Bandel, Emilia Soroko, Marcin Sękowski
Developmental Psychology, Volume 28, Issue 2, 2023, pp. 63-79
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.23.016.19853René Wodarz, Aleksandra Rogowska
Developmental Psychology, Volume 28, Issue 2, 2023, pp. 81-97
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.23.017.19854Anna Szymanik-Kostrzewska
Developmental Psychology, Volume 28, Issue 2, 2023, pp. 99-114
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.23.018.19855Michalina Dzielińska, Marta Lasota, Konrad Piotrowski
Developmental Psychology, Volume 28, Issue 2, 2023, pp. 115-133
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.23.019.19856The purpose of this study was to assess the severity of parental burnout among parents of children with disabilities and chronic illness. It was hypothesized that these parents would be characterized by higher levels of parental burnout and that their child’s disability and chronic illness would moderate the relationship of parental burnout with sociodemographic factors. The study was conducted with 254 parents (Mage = 35.37 years; SDage = 5.80) with children aged one month to 35 years; 22% of parents had a child with a disability or chronic illness. The Parental Burnout Assessment (PBA) questionnaire was used to verify the research hypotheses. The correlation analyses showed that parents of children with disabilities had higher levels of parental burnout, and as the degree of constraints related to the child’s difficulties increased, the level of parental burnout increased. The moderation analyses proved to be statistically significant for the relationship between the number of children and parental burnout. Similar relationships were not found concerning the relationship of parental burnout with the fact of having a child under five or the gender of the parent. The study indicated that there are relationships between having a child with a disability or chronic illness, the degree of their limitations, and parental burnout.
Michalina Dzielińska, Marta Lasota, Konrad Piotrowski
Developmental Psychology, Volume 28, Issue 2, 2023, pp. 135-152
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.23.021.19914The purpose of this study was to assess the severity of parental burnout among parents of children with disabilities and chronic illness. It was hypothesized that parents of children with disabilites/ilnesses would be characterized by higher levels of parental burnout and that their child’s disability and chronic illness would moderate the relationship of parental burnout with sociodemographic factors. The study was conducted with 254 parents (Mage = 35.37; SDage = 5.80) with children aged from 1 month to 35 years, 22% of whom had a child with a disability or chronic illness. The Parental Burnout Assessment (PBA) questionnaire was used to verify the research hypotheses. The correlation analyses conducted showed that parents of children with disabilities had higher levels of parental burnout, and as the degree of constraints related to the child’s difficulties increased, the level of parental burnout increased. The results of moderation analyses showed that the relationship between the number of children and parental burnout was statistically significant. Similar relationships were not found concerning the relationship of parental burnout and having a child under five or the gender of the parent. The study indicates that there are relationships between having a child with a disability or chronic illness, the degree of their impairment, and parental burnout.
Małgorzata Stępień-Nycz
Developmental Psychology, Volume 28, Issue 2, 2023, pp. 153-155
Publication date: 31.12.2023
Editor-in-Chief: Maria Kielar-Turska
This publication has been supported by The Polish Association of Human Development Psychology.
Maria Kielar-Turska
Developmental Psychology, Volume 28 Issue 1, 2023, pp. 11-24
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.23.001.18176The paper presents Stefan Szuman as a multidimensional person both in his personal life and academic work. He was characterised by his academic knowledge in several areas: medicine, psychology and art. He communicated with professionals from various fields – philosophy, pedagogy, literary studies, linguistics and musicology – such as: Witkacy, Bruno Schulz or Jan Puget. In his research, he was interested in topics related to mental development (the essence of development; developmental factors; cognitive, emotional, personality changes), education, teaching (students’ attention, teaching skills) and communing with art. He used different research methods: observation, conversation, quasi-experiment and tests. He presented the results of his researches using appropriate and sophisticated language. The paper shows the current remembrance of Szuman and his works both through public events (e.g. art exhibitions) and conferences.
Leszek Sosnowski
Developmental Psychology, Volume 28 Issue 1, 2023, pp. 25-36
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.23.002.18177The main purpose of the article is to present the correspondence between Ingarden and Szuman, which necessarily requires including Witkacy in the discussion as a link between the scholars. The article has been divided into several parts, which cover the pre-war period, the time of war and the post-war era. Each of the periods had its own thematic specificity; the first discusses personal matters, the second scientific ones, and the third scientific-administrative ones.
Adam Niemczyński
Developmental Psychology, Volume 28 Issue 1, 2023, pp. 37-53
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.23.003.18178At the beginning of his scientific career Szuman (1930a) indicted the individual personality development to be the object of his psychological investigations. He put a stress on historical character of the personality development and the role of significant deeds in it. He placed psychology of human development among humanistic disciplines of investigation in his book on human personality and character (Szuman 2014). Psychology of child and adolescent development also belong to them and are irreducible to the development of human biological organism. Child’s own actions and upbringing as well as education branch the development of individual human beings out of biology without detaching it from biology nonetheless. Szuman’s historical psychology of the individual personal development did not completely abandon a stance represented in the theory of cognition called the British empiricism, although he clearly put aside positivistic philosophy of science. It is necessary to emancipate historical psychology from the obstacles of the British empiricism’s theory of cognition in order to continue its progress. Niemczyński (2017) seems to go in this direction.
Dorota Kubicka
Developmental Psychology, Volume 28 Issue 1, 2023, pp. 55-60
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.23.004.18179Urlic Neisser (1928–2012) is widely considered to be one of the leading figures of contemporary psychology. This scholar is also thought to have come up with an original idea concerning the constructive nature of the perception process referred to as Neisser’s cycle of perception (1976; 1978). The thesis has contributed to the development of scientific psychology, but few notice that the idea ascribed to Neisser had been developed much earlier by Stefan Szuman (1889–1972) in his concept known as the genesis of the object (1932). Comparing the two concepts makes one conclude that they are strikingly similar and that novel ideas originating on the fringes of a given field are not given enough credit.
Dorota Kubicka
Developmental Psychology, Volume 28 Issue 1, 2023, pp. 61-66
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.23.007.19302Urlic Neisser (1928–2012) is widely considered to be one of the leading figures of contemporary psychology. This scholar is also thought to have come up with an original idea concerning the constructive nature of the perception process referred to as Neisser’s cycle of perception (1976, 1978). The thesis has contributed to the development of scientific psychology, but few notice that the idea ascribed to Neisser had been developed much earlier by Stefan Szuman (1889–1972) in his concept known as the genesis of the object (1932). Comparing the two concepts makes one reach the conclusion that they are strikingly similar and that novel ideas originating on the fringes of a given field are not given enough credit.
Lech Witkowski
Developmental Psychology, Volume 28 Issue 1, 2023, pp. 67-75
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.23.005.18180The article points to the original strategy of reading Stefan Szuman’s works, which has already been documented in monographic studies by the author of this text. Szuman’s classical achievement in the field of Polish psychology studies has been put into the context of the phenomenon of the „duality turn-over”, which came to theoretical maturity through the works of the „Great Generation” of the Polish pedagogues of the Interwar period, in which the author situates Szuman’s research involvements and his achievements. The paper recalls the chosen aspects, extracted from the psychologist’s works thanks to the above mentioned approach of the author. Both Szuman’s ingenious intuition and drive as well as the „epistemological obstacles” of the thought tradition in the psychology of his time – from the first decades of 20th century – are emphasised here.
Anna Grabowska, Grażyna Jackowska
Developmental Psychology, Volume 28 Issue 1, 2023, pp. 77-95
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.23.006.18181The paper presents aspects of Stefan Szuman’s activities and passions that influenced both his scientific interests and friendships. He was involved in various forms of fine arts: painting, wood carving, hand weaving, poetry writing and joint music making in small home bands. For both him and Zofia Szuman – his beloved wife – aesthetic impressions were extremely important for building up their lifestyle and creating relationships with others, including famous Polish painter – Stanisław Ignacy Witkiewicz (Witkacy), philosopher – Roman Ingarden and writer – Bruno Schulz. The paper references the old family correspondence which creates genuine and trustworthy foundation for the image of their lives.
Publication date: 2023
Editor-in-Chief: Maria Kielar-Turska
Issue Editor: Dorota Kubicka
This publication has been supported by The Polish Association of Human Development Psychology.
Marta Krasuska-Betiuk
Developmental Psychology, Volume 27 Issue 4, 2022, pp. 9-23
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.22.021.18080The purpose of the text is to point out the elements linking social representation theory and developmental psychology. Despite the differences that may seem obvious between developmental psychology and social psychology, in reality there is a deep bond that unites them; a continuity, resulting from the notion that both sub-disciplines explore issues of knowledge transformation: the former studies the transformation of knowledge over time, the latter over space. It seems interesting to show the connections and differences between these elements of the theories of Jean Piaget, Lev Vygotsky and Serge Moscovici, which deal with the concept of the development of children’s knowledge of the world in a socio-cultural context. In addition, the epistemology of social representations developed by Gerald Duveen on the basis of Piaget’s concept is important for showing these connections.
Paulina Anikiej-Wiczenbach, Arkadiusz Mański
Developmental Psychology, Volume 27 Issue 4, 2022, pp. 25-35
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.22.022.18081Duchenne Muscular Dystrophy is a disease that shows extremely complex connections between the genetic-molecular sphere and the characteristics of psychological functioning. The text presented below introduces the importance of biological sources in explaining many aspects of the psychological functioning of DMD patients. In the field of research and rehabilitation practice related to DMD, interdisciplinarity becomes a necessity that can improve the quality of life of patients with DMD and their families to a greater extent than before. On the one hand, the article is a concise account of the views that have been dominating both in the past and today about this disease, as well as a proposal outlining the directions of future research related to describing and explaining the processes that form the psychological image of people with DMD. The complexity of this image can be made more accessible and understandable if, both in research and in everyday life, it becomes possible to refer to the genetic-molecular sources, psychological resources and characteristics of the environment in which a person with DMD grows up in the process of describing and explaining DMD.
Danuta Borecka-Biernat
Developmental Psychology, Volume 27 Issue 4, 2022, pp. 39-55
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.22.023.18082The aim of the study was to identify family-related factors connected with aggressive and task-oriented coping strategies in adolescent youth in situations of social conflict. The study was conducted on a sample of 893 adolescents aged 13 to 15. M. Plopa’s Parental Attitude Scale (Skala Postaw Rodzicielskich, SPR), the Questionnaire for Coping Strategies in Parents in Situations of Social Conflict (Kwestionariusz Strategii Radzenia sobie Rodziców w Sytuacji Konfliktu Społecznego, KSRwSK) by D. Borecka-Biernat, and Questionnaire for Coping Strategies in Adolescents in Situations of Social Conflict (Kwestionariusz do Badania Strategii Radzenia sobie Młodzieży w Sytuacji Konfliktu Społecznego, KSMK) by D. Borecka-Biernat were used in the study. An analysis of the results of the study has indicated family discriminants of aggressive and task-oriented coping strategies in adolescents in situations of social conflict; those discriminants include the perceived parental attitudes in father, including the inconsistent, autonomous, and demanding attitude; the perceived aggressive strategy in mother and father in situations of social conflict, as well as the task-oriented strategy in mother in situations of social conflict. Identifying the determinants of aggressive and task-oriented strategies in youth might allow the caretakers of adolescents to better understand their behaviour in situations of social conflict and look at its roots from the young people’s perspective. Understanding the determinants of these behaviours is a necessary step in designing strategies that can shape individuals capable of coping with conflicts in a mature manner.
Karolina Dworska, Jakub Romaneczko
Developmental Psychology, Volume 27 Issue 4, 2022, pp. 57-69
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.22.024.18083In the early years of life, motor skills contribute to the formation of cognitive skills. The aim o the present study (conducted in June 2021) was to determine mutual relationships between motor variables: aiming-catching as well as manual dexterity, and the cognitive variable of spatial reasoning. It was assumed that manual dexterity played a mediating role in the relationship between agility skills and spatial reasoning. A second aim of study was to test the assumption that age played a moderating role in the relationship between manual dexterity and spatial reasoning. The participants were Polish preschool children (N = 83), including 42 boys (100% white race of children from Eastern Poland). The results indicated that manual dexterity mediated the relationship between aiming-catching skills and spatial reasoning. Age as an important moderator of the relationship between manual dexterity and spatial reasoning.
Łukasz Nikel
Developmental Psychology, Volume 27 Issue 4, 2022, pp. 71-84
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.22.025.18084The goal of the current study was to explore the relations between the Big Five personality traits and school achievements of children in the fourth grade of primary school. Participants were (n = 157) children (aged 9–11 years) attending public primary school in Poland. Children completed a self-description inventory for the Big Five and teachers evaluated current school achievements of children for mathematics and Polish language. The obtained results indicated that neuroticism and openness to experiences were significantly associated with school achievements. Moreover, the relationship between the Big Five and school achievements was moderated by gender in that a) agreeableness as well as the Big Five sub-scales of inferiority and helping behaviour were negatively associated with school achievements only among girls, and b) the openness to experience sub-scale of problem solver was positively associated with school achievements only among boys. The results suggest that the relationship between the Big Five personality traits and school achievements may depend on gender.
Joanna Kossewska, Katarzyna Tomaszek, Emilia Macałka, Gabriela Początek
Developmental Psychology, Volume 27 Issue 4, 2022, pp. 85-99
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.22.026.18085High school graduates usually suffer from stress one month before the final exams. The research’s aim was to compare the depression and burnout symptoms measured in April 2019 (before the pandemic) and in April 2020 (during the pandemic) in the Polish high school students. The results showed that the COVID-19 pandemic affected adolescent girls more than boys, leading to higher rates of depression, student burnout, and imbalanced time perspective. Females perceived lower family acceptance than boys. Males were less depressed and more burned out during the pandemic than before, while girls not only were more burned out but were also more depressed, were imbalanced in their time perspective, and perceived lower family acceptance during the COVID-19 pandemic than before the outbreak.
Publication date: 2023
Editor-in-Chief: Maria Kielar-Turska
Editors of the issue: Dorota Czyżowska
This publication has been supported by The Polish Association of Human Development Psychology.
Konrad Piotrowski, Michalina Dzielińska, Katarzyna Sanna, Dorota Szczygieł
Developmental Psychology, Volume 27 Issue 3, 2022, pp. 9-23
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.22.015.17676The aim of the article is to present the concept of parental burnout, which has recently become a topic of interest for researchers. In the article, we review the theoretical concept of pa-rental burnout, present the questionnaires to measure it as well as the research results indicating risk factors and consequences of parental burnout, and the first studies on the effectiveness of psychological interventions to reduce it. This article presents information in a concise, simple and understandable way, providing readers with a quick overview of the theory of parental burnout and the latest research on this phenomenon.
Zofia Dołęga
Developmental Psychology, Volume 27 Issue 3, 2022, pp. 25-35
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.22.016.17677The article is devoted to the characteristics of a course of the psychological crisis caused by a critical event, which is the death of a child. The author of the work refers to the cyclical-phase model of developmental changes, which is well-known in the developmental psychology, and the Dual Process Model of Coping with Bereavement (DPM) of Margaret Stroebe, Henk Schut and Catrin Finkenauer (1999, 2001, 2010, 2013, 2015). What parents experience after the loss of a child is not an identical and parallel process. The course of mourning in parents is not the same. Rather, it is the interweaving of different states of mourning, sometimes with a regression to previous states. The result is a different mental condition of the mother and the father as a consequence of dealing more or less successfully with the loss of a child. The paper, based on theoretical and empirical analyses, proposes an original model of four psychological statuses of parents after the loss of a child. It takes into account the differentiation of the mental condition of parents in connection with the possibility of adequate psychological help. The bereavement status model (MSK) takes into consideration the statuses of secondary adaptation, unfinished mourning, complex persistent bereavement disorder, and the status of possible post-traumatic growth. A hypothesis worthy of empirical verification has been formulated that in assessing the mental condition of parents after the loss of a child, it is worth proposing a specific diagnostic package containing information on the life situation of the family; the time that has passed since the death of a child; the sex of the parent; intensification of feelings of loneliness, availability of social support and its perception by parents, and the assessment of coping strategies for everyday stress. It remains an open question, what are the direct and indirect relationships between these variables and whether their differentiation proves the legitimacy of distinguishing certain conditioning statuses of mourning in parents after the loss of a child. This is currently the subject of an empirical research testing out the MSK model.
Joanna Matuszczak-Świgoń
Developmental Psychology, Volume 27 Issue 3, 2022, pp. 37-47
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.22.017.17678Since 1930, there has been a shift from an interest only in the resilience of the individual to analyzing it in relation to the family as well. Family resilience is a process of positive adaptation of the entire family and its members, and effective coping with life’s adversities (Luthar et al., 2000). In addition to coping with the demands, family resilience enables development in the face of serious life challenges (Walsh, 2006, 2016). Using the key processes of resilience, a family facing difficulties can emerge stronger and more resourceful in the face of future challenges. Froma Walsh (2013) distinguishes three overarching domains of family resilience: the belief system, the organizational patterns of family life, and the communication and problem-solving process. Research on the post-traumatic growth indicates that a crisis can be a serious warning that increases family’s attention to the core values and important life issues; an opportunity to reassess life priorities that initiates the allocation of more time and attention to meaningful relationships (Tedeschi, Calhoun, 2004). The article presents the definitions and the history of the concept of family resilience. The Walsh model of family resilience is also presented and the practical implications of the model are discussed.
Joanna Siudem-Kuna, Anna Cierpka
Developmental Psychology, Volume 27 Issue 3, 2022, pp. 51-71
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.22.018.17679For the last several decades, psychologists have been paying attention to the changes in the Polish family model and the transformation of the father’s role within it. These are the processes that were also significantly affected by the COVID-19 pandemic. The objective of the research was to understand how the Polish young men awaiting the birth of their first child experience fatherhood in this time of crisis. Seven semi-structured interviews were conducted with men aged 26–35. Their transcripts were subject to Interpretative Phenomenological Analysis (IPA). The results showed that the pandemic took away the opportunity to act in areas that were previously available to fathers from the participants. At the same time, the pandemic gave them new areas where they could fulfil themselves as fathers. Despite the limited access to prenatal care services, participants got a chance to be more present in their unborn child’s life. Moreover, they got a chance to be more active at the stage of waiting for their birth.
Dorota Katarzyna Suwalska-Barancewicz, Alicja Malina
Developmental Psychology, Volume 27 Issue 3, 2022, pp. 73-84
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.22.019.17680Suppressing negative emotions (anger, fear, sadness) can lead to an increase in emotional tension. Unexpressed emotions contribute to the deterioration of health and may also contribute to the emergence of psychosomatic disorders and certain diseases, such as coronary artery disease, hypertension, and cancer. Research shows that depressive emotionality is associated with lower self-esteem while lowering life quality and life satisfaction. The aim of the study was to analyze whether suppressing negative emotions is related to the level of self-esteem amongst young mothers and their satisfaction with life, as well as to identify mediators that determine the quality of this relationship. 340 young mothers (mean age 30.09) were examined. To conduct the research the Emotional Control Scale – CECS in its Polish adaptation of Zygfryd Juczyński, the Life Satisfaction Scale by Zygfryd Juczyński and the SES Self-esteem Scale adapted by Mariola Łaguna, Kinga Lachowicz-Tabaczek and Irena Dzwonkowska were used. The analyses confirmed the existence of significant relations between the level of suppressing negative emotions (sadness and anxiety) and self-esteem as well as life satisfaction. It also turned out that the level of self-esteem is an important mediator in the relation between the suppression of sadness and anxiety by young mothers and their satisfaction with life.
Bożena Gulla, Kinga Tucholska, Agnieszka Ziernicka-Wojtaszek
Developmental Psychology, Volume 27 Issue 3, 2022, pp. 85-99
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.22.020.17681Children and adolescents are the groups particularly vulnerable to the consequences of the climate crisis. Global warming, extreme weather phenomena, and progressive environmental degradation have an adverse effect on their development. It is up to the adults to make decisions and actions allowing for mitigating the consequences of climate change, opting for an environmentally friendly household management, offering protection and support to the children, as well as explaining the situation to them and shaping their attitudes. That is why the presented research focuses precisely on parents, the specificity of their functioning in the climate crisis compared to childless people. The study included a group of 333 adults, including 67 parents. Self-report methods were used, including questionnaires developed specifically for this project that examined knowledge about the climate and belief in climate myths; as well as the inventories on current and planned pro-ecological activity. The proprietary scale examining climate emotions and the Climate Change Anxiety Scale by Clayton and Karazsia were also used. The relationships between the variables established in the group of parents and the differences between the groups of people who are parents and those who do not have children were analyzed. Based on the results of the study, an attempt was made to analyze the experiences and behaviours of parents in the context of the climate crisis, and a number of guidelines were formulated that can help them in dealing with children so that they experience the climate situation in the least burdensome way possible, while at the same time receiving support from adults, creating habits that are good for the climate and building up the motivation for pro-environmental activity.
Publication date: 2022
Editor-in-Chief: Maria Kielar-Turska
Editors of the issue: Joanna Kossewska, Wioletta Radziwiłłowicz
This publication has been supported by The Polish Association of Human Development Psychology.
Marek Szurpita, Wioletta Radziwiłłowicz
Developmental Psychology, Volume 27 Issue 2, 2022, pp. 9-25
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.22.008.17465The article is an attempt to analyze the contemporary results of research on neurodevelopmental disorders in order to advance the search for various diagnostic approaches, predictors of a negative course of development, and changes in the dynamics of symptoms. The authors, based on a literature review, show that different levels of susceptibility to negative events are not a simple consequence of a specific NDD diagnosis [i.e., autism spectrum disorders (ASD) or ADHD] but also depend on the interaction of genetic factors (i.e., the general phenotype of NDD, which depends on spontaneous genetic mutations or changes in the number of copies of DNA fragments) and prenatal and perinatal environmental factors.
Anna Sumiła, Łukasz Szostakiewicz, Marta Bogotko, Małgorzata Pawłowicz
Developmental Psychology, Volume 27 Issue 2, 2022, pp. 27-41
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.22.009.17466In recent years, there has been a significant increase in the number of diagnosed cases of autism. In the case of a correctly made diagnosis, a positive effect of this phenomenon is the possibility of obtaining therapeutic intervention that is adequate for the patient. On the other hand, some researchers note the risk associated with overdiagnosis of these conditions. It is postulated that the diagnosis process should not focus only on the presence of individual symptoms, but also on understanding their meaning and the history of their occurrence. In addition, many mental, neurological and genetic disorders may have symptoms that are common to or mimic those of autism. A diagnosis of autism has specific psychological (immediate), social and financial effects, and incorrect diagnosis can delay the best intervention for the patient.
Joanna Szczepańska
Developmental Psychology, Volume 27 Issue 2, 2022, pp. 45-55
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.22.010.17467As the population of children who are diagnosed as being on autism spectrum grows in number, so does the interest of scientists in this subject. Specialists in various fields increasingly not only focus on the children themselves but also examine their families’ situation and functioning. By doing so, they aim to understand the family as a system. Numerous empirical studies have demonstrated that when a baby with a disability is born or when it develops in an abnormal way, this becomes a source of suffering, anxiety, grief, or hostility towards the outside world. Such a situation is viewed as a crisis, a loss, and a process of mourning; working through these experiences is difficult, requires tremendous work, and requires external support in many cases. For these reasons, this paper provides a brief overview of parents’ experiences in the process of diagnosing a child; it cites the literature on the topic and shows the different paths of these experiences.
Emma Nawrocka, Kacper Gargul, Adam Mańkowski, Aleksandra Mańkowska
Developmental Psychology, Volume 27 Issue 2, 2022, pp. 57-69
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.22.011.17468Autism spectrum disorders (ASD) are disorders with a neurodevelopmental background. Their characteristic features are stereotypical: constant and repetitive patterns of behavior and interests, as well as difficulties in social communication and interpersonal relations. Research works on adolescents with ASD show that building relationships with other people is a challenge for them because – even though they might have very good general knowledge of social principles – they have insufficiently developed skills to recognize signals in human behavior and to respond adequately to them. Moreover, these difficulties in the emotional and social spheres are initially often overlooked in children who are on the autism spectrum but function on an average or high level. The main aim of the work is to present the situation of two adolescent brothers who both present the symptoms of Asperger’s syndrome (AS). At the time of the examination, the younger boy (Wojtek) was 14 years old and had been diagnosed with Asperger’s Syndrome. The older brother (Tomek) was 18 years old and had started a series of meetings with a psychologist. Both teenagers were tested with tools that assess the development of social competencies, intelligence and emotional intelligence. The conducted tests and interviews showed that the boy without a confirmed diagnosis of Asperger’s Syndrome had greater difficulties than his younger brother in understanding the emotions and intentions of other people. The younger brother felt much better in the interpersonal area and functioned more effectively in social situations, despite the fact that he also has intra- and interpersonal problems. Perhaps due to his timely diagnosis, the younger brother obtains more support from his family and probably also received support at key moments in his social and emotional development. The presented case study emphasizes the importance of the proper socio-emotional assessment of people in the diagnostic process. Its limitations are the lack of assessment of the boys’ mood and temperamental differences. These issues should be addressed in subsequent research.
Joanna Kossewska, Magdalena Milczarek
Developmental Psychology, Volume 27 Issue 2, 2022, pp. 71-87
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.22.012.17469People with autism spectrum condition (ASC) sometimes mask the features of autism, and this could be related to their level of self-esteem. The aim of the research was to show the relationship between the intensity of autistic features, camouflage and self-esteem. Polish versions of the following tools were used in the study: the Short Autism Spectrum Quotient Test (AQ-10), the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale, and the Camouflaging Autistic Traits Questionnaire-CAT-Q. The study involved 100 adults (aged 18 to 52) who were divided into two groups based on their ASD diagnosis and empirical AQ-10 indicators: an ASD group (N1 = 41, declaration of ASD diagnosis and indicator AQ ≥ 6; 31 females, 4 males, 6 nonbinary persons, mean age = 28.71, sd = 8.86); and a non-ASD group (N2 = 48, no ASD diagnosis and indicator AQ 6; 29 females, 17 males and nonbinary persons, mean age = 26.14, sd = 9.64). The results revealed the existence of a linear relationship between the severity of autistic traits and the use of camouflaging strategies. No linear relationship was found between self-esteem and the other variables; however, some significant differentiation was identified due to the distinguished level of self-esteem (low, medium, high).
Natalia Józefacka
Developmental Psychology, Volume 27 Issue 2, 2022, pp. 89-98
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.22.013.17470Introduction: Executive functions have been the subject of intensive research in recent years. Both questionnaire tools and experimental tasks are used to measure them. A review of the literature on the subject indicates a significant but weak relationship between these two methods of measurement. It is worth emphasizing that, in the case of children, questionnaire measurement is very prone to errors caused by differences between indirect observation by parents or teachers. Aim: The aim of this article is to increase the current knowledge of the relationship between questionnaire measures and experimental tasks. In particular, this study aims to investigate differences between the observations of parents and teachers in the assessment of working memory and inhibition in children. The relationship between experimental and questionnaire tasks is studied, and a hypothesis regarding a stronger relationship between these two methods of measurement using latent variables is verified. Study group: The study was conducted on a group of 200 children, aged 5–6, attending preschool or the first grade of primary school. Result: In the analyzed group, the differences between the answers of teachers and parents were significant, but the effect size was low. The relationship between the individual subscales was also moderate. The relationship between questionnaire measurements and experimental tasks in the study group has been confirmed by other studies. The correlation coefficient between parents’ and teachers’ assessments is significant and low, ranging from 0.19 to 0.30. Conclusions: In conclusion, the obtained results confirm the observations of other researchers: task-based and questionnaire-based measures evaluate different constructs. Questionnaires capture more global expressions of executive functions in different life contexts (home, school) over longer periods of time, while experimental tasks assess basic cognitive abilities in a more predictable and structured environment.
Marta Orańska-Szałomska
Developmental Psychology, Volume 27 Issue 2, 2022, pp. 101-102
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.22.014.17471Publication date: 13.12.2022
Editor-in-Chief: Maria Kielar-Turska
Issue Editors: Ludmiła Zając-Lamparska, Małgorzata Stępień-Nycz
This publication has been supported by The Polish Association of Human Development Psychology.
Agnieszka Franczok-Kuczmowska
Developmental Psychology, Volume 27 Issue 1, 2022, pp. 9-26
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.22.001.16676The aim of the article is to present different approaches to understanding resilience as well as to point out possibilities for research in this area. A broad list of measurement tools for researching adults in terms of resilience, resilience related factors and mental health is presented. Examples of standards and criteria are presented, which can be helpful in the process of constructing further studies, with the aim of finding a common field of research and further possibility of generalizing conclusions. The article also presents examples of longitudinal research and research conducted in the experimental paradigm. It concludes with the necessity for further empirical and applied research on resilience, especially involving the three levels: cognitive, emotional and behavioural.
Ifunanya Chukwueke, Mary Basil Nwoke, John Eze
Developmental Psychology, Volume 27 Issue 1, 2022, pp. 29-37
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.22.002.16677Choosing a career path is an important decision made in the course of life, a developmental task which every individual faces. Career indecision, however, is a challenge that has plagued young people who do not know the career options they have, or lack vital information on oneself that will help them make a course choice. Some factors that play a role in career decision including career guidance, interest-based personality and systemizing were investigated to predict students’ course choice. The study involved 248 participants who completed the survey online. Results revealed that both the utility and deficiency subscales of career guidance did not predict the course choice. Enterprising personality type predicted course choice only when career guidance and interest-based personality were considered (OR = 6.36, p = .03, 95% CI = [1.11, 36.41]). However, topography subscale of systemizing predicted course choice, noting a decreasing odds of falling within the humanities category (OR = .64, p = .01, 95% CI = [.44, .91]). The study shows that career indecision is still one of the major developmental challenges that an individual faces while growing up, thus career counsellors are expected to play a vital role in providing solutions to these challenges by providing insightful guidance to individuals from an early stage.
Kamil Janowicz
Developmental Psychology, Volume 27 Issue 1, 2022, pp. 39-65
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.22.003.16678The last decades brought us a deeper reflection on the role of the future and thinking about it in our lives in psychology. Numerous studies supplied us with much empirical evidence on how crucial the role of thinking about one’s future is to human behavior and to general development. Making predictions about one’s future is one type of prospective activity. These predictions (vision) may be related to many areas of life, e.g., intimate relationship, work, being a parent. The current study aimed to develop a quantitative, self-report measure of the extension of the vision of own parenting (VOP). Findings from the first study (N = 450, M [age in years] = 20.82, SD = 2.81) resulted in reducing initial 105-item version to the 85-item one. Findings from the second study, with a different sample (N = 352, M [age in years] = 19.57, SD = 2.48), resulted in 78-item (divided into eleven scales) version of the Vision of Own Parenting Questionnaire (VOPQ). These results were supported by confirmatory factor analysis. The internal reliability of the VOPQ was assessed across two studies, and was found to be high (α = .72 - .89). The criterion validity was established in the second study, whereby the VOPQ subscales were demonstrated to be associated with future time perspective, empathy, and life values. The VOPQ represents a valid, psychometrically sound, and novel method of assessing the extension of the vision of own parenting and parenting valuing.
Sanja Smojver-Ažić, Katarina Banov Trošelj
Developmental Psychology, Volume 27 Issue 1, 2022, pp. 67-77
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.22.004.16679The role of attachment and filial maturity dimensions in predicting expectations of filial caregiving was investigated on a sample of 296 (186 females) prospective caregivers aged 26 to 60. Consistent with previous studies, attachment avoidance negatively predicted the filial caregiving expectations. These effects were partially mediated by filial comprehending, which predicted higher self-expectations in providing assistance to the parent. Filial distancing was proven to be a negative predictor of caregiving expectations, although in low relation with attachment avoidance. Findings suggest the importance of further exploration, as well as the importance of the concept of filial maturity in the context of filial responsibilities and long-term parent-child relations.
Anna Szymanik-Kostrzewska
Developmental Psychology, Volume 27 Issue 1, 2022, pp. 79-97
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.22.005.16680The pattern of the functioning of a Polish mother, based on the cognitive component of her stereotype shared by mothers, combines certain features, behaviors and devotion to the family. “Parenting myths”are parents’beliefs about the necessity to give their child all the best and ensure a happy childhood. It was assumed, that both self-sacrificing for children and being guided by myths can lead to women’s overburdening with parental role. Three methods were used: the Identification with a Polish Mother Questionnaire, the Selected Parental Myths Questionnaire and Polish version of the Parental Burnout Assessment. 284 mothers (aged 19–55) took part in the research. Preliminary results showed that the degree of identification with the characteristics of the Polish-Mother was negatively correlated with parental burnout symptoms, but positively with following parental myths.
Krzysztof Gerc, Iwona Sikorska, Magdalena Łozińska
Developmental Psychology, Volume 27 Issue 1, 2022, pp. 99-115
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.22.006.16681The theory of selective optimization with compensation (SOC), developed by Paul Baltes, and a relational position in the psychological analysis of stress were chosen as the basis for the assumptions of the research study reported on in this article. Additionally, it was assumed that a sense of well-being in seniors may be reduced by crises, which are a response to difficult and unexpected life events. The purpose of this study was to identify the relationship between selected strategies of coping with stress and the well-being of seniors during the period of forced social isolation that was introduced in the spring of 2020 during the first wave of the COVID-19 pandemic.
The participants of this on-line study consisted of 190 late adulthood individuals with a mean age of 68.7 (SD = 5.24). The measurement tools included assessment of the following areas: perceived stress (PSS-10), ways of coping with stress (MINI COPE), the sense of efficacy (GSES) and life satisfaction (SWLS).
The results showed that the most common coping methods in the group of seniors studied were active coping, planning, positive revaluation and acceptance. Correlation-regression analysis showed that the level of stress of seniors during a pandemic negatively depends on a sense of efficacy and two ways of coping with stress which include acceptance and active coping, and positively depends on a denial and psychoactive substance use.
These results are predominantly consistent with other research studies reported during the pandemic period, which indicate the directions of possible preventive as well as intervention measures for seniors.
Gabriela Gore-Gorszewska
Developmental Psychology, Volume 27 Issue 1, 2022, pp. 119-123
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.22.007.16682Publication date: 03.2022
Editor-in-Chief: Maria Kielar-Turska
Issue Editor: Maria Kielar-Turska
Czasopismo wydawane dzięki dotacji Polskiego Stowarzyszenia Psychologii Rozwoju Człowieka
Anna Filip, Anna Ochał-Zalewska
Developmental Psychology, Volume 26 Issue 4, 2021, pp. 9-21
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.21.024.15480Preterm infants are at increased risk of difficulties in language development, including speech and language developmental delays. Preterm birth impacts the pattern and the quality of parent–child interactions, which, in turn, may affect the child’s language and communication development. The article presents the results of research on parents’interactions with their preterm born children and describes some programs aimed at improving parent–child interactions.
Dorota Kubacka-Jasiecka
Developmental Psychology, Volume 26 Issue 4, 2021, pp. 23-41
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.21.025.15481This work, inspired by the symbolic “centenary”of Polish psychology has been dedicated to the emergence and development of applied psychology. The main protagonist of applied psychology, and clinical psychology in particular, has been, in the author’s opinion, Lightner Witmer (1867–1956), an American psychologist promoting the clinical method of getting to know the patient through a direct contact of the psychologist with the maladjusted, including contact in their everyday environment. Witmer’s message, supported by the new ideas of the systems theory, environmental psychology and the spreading concepts of social psychology, can be all found in what is currently expected from psychologists. Applied psychology is not only a “bridge”between the theory of psychology and professional practice, but most of all, it remains a science directed towards practice, open to the contemporary challenges; a science that accepts the necessity to adjust the ethos of the profession and the new tasks of environmental work for the improvement of health and the quality of life.
Paulina Buczek, Katarzyna Tomaszek
Developmental Psychology, Volume 26 Issue 4, 2021, pp. 45-59
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.21.026.15482This study examined the role of health behaviors in the relationship between personality traits and psychological well-being. The sample consisted of 147 participants (51% female), aged between 20 and 35 years. Respondents completed the NEO-FFI Personality Inventory, Psychological Well-being Scale (PWBS), and Health Behaviour Inventory (IZZ). The results confirmed the significant relationships between the personality traits, health behaviors and psychological well-being. Extraversion, conscientiousness and neuroticism were related to health behaviors. Personality traits appeared as significant predictors of psychological well-being (68% of explained variances in PWBS). The health behaviors mediated the relationship between three abovementioned personality traits and psychological well-being. A higher health behavior score was shown to reduce the strength of the effect of personality traits on psychological well-being. Findings indicate that personality traits are important in designing health programs to improve mental health.
Hagit Nagar-Shimoni, Shahar Gindi, Ya’arit Bokek-Cohen, Yael Leitner
Developmental Psychology, Volume 26 Issue 4, 2021, pp. 61-66
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.21.027.15483The aim of this report is to call attention to a unique clinical group of children who are not diagnosed with ASD but exhibit excessive interest in specific topics or repetitive behaviors. It is argued that this group might require specific developmental stimulation, especially in light of the fact that it does not receive adequate professional treatment or support. The results of conducted surveys are supplemented with additional diagnostic and anamnestic data from which practical conclusions based on research evidence are drawn and general guidelines for intervention are formulated. Further research into this group is also suggested.
Maja Moroch, Dorota Katarzyna Suwalska-Barancewicz
Developmental Psychology, Volume 26 Issue 4, 2021, pp. 67-84
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.21.028.15484Theoretical background: Personal resources are one of the attributes that protect against the destructive consequences of experiencing a difficult situation and that figure in the overall assessment of life satisfaction.
Aim: The aim of the study was to analyze whether the individual dimensions of attachment are related to the level of personal resources of people diagnosed with schizophrenia and their life satisfaction, and to identify mediators that determine the quality of these relationships.
Study group: 50 people were tested. The average age of the respondents was 49 years.
Method: The measurement of the variable attachment was made using Experiences in Close Relationships-Revised by Chris Fraley, Kelly Brennan and Neils Waller, in the Polish adaptation of Cezary Wiśniewski; the Life Satisfaction Scale by Zygfryd Juczyński was used to determine the variable satisfaction with life. The level of personal resources was measured using the Lifespan Individual Resilience Scale in the Polish adaptation of Alicja Malina, Julie Ann Pooley and Craig Harms.
Results: The analyzes carried out in the course of the study confirmed the existence of significant relationships between the level of personal resources and life satisfaction. Research has also confirmed that there are significant links between attachment and life satisfaction, and between attachment and personal resources. It also turned out that the level of personal resources is an important mediator in the relation between avoidance and the level of life satisfaction in people diagnosed with schizophrenia.
Conclusion: The level of personal resources and secure attachment correlate with a higher life satisfaction of people diagnosed with schizophrenia.
Grażyna Mendecka
Developmental Psychology, Volume 26 Issue 4, 2021, pp. 85-101
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.21.029.15485The analysis of gender identity is based on the narratives of women who volunteered to serve in the military after the start of World War II and fought as soldiers in the Red Army. Forty years after World War II, Svetlana Alexievich conducted interviews with women veterans, which she published in the book War’s Unwomanly Face. Their memories of wartime and post-war period were analyzed from the perspective of their gender identity, i.e. the ability to reconcile the role of a woman determined by their biological sex with the role of a soldier determined by the circumstances. The interpretation of this problem offered in the paper is based on developmental psychology theories of Erik Erikson, James Marcia, Daniel Levinson, and Jeffrey Arnett, and the sociological perspective on identity. Selected narratives from Alexievich’s reportages are analyzed focusing on identification and interpretation of different themes, which are assessed according to their relevance to the understanding of the process described. The narrators were only 16–20 years old at the time of joining the military and they were still at the stage of identity moratorium.
It required a lot of determination for them to become a soldier. Their identity as a soldier was their assumed identity, defined by Marcia as ideological or professional engagement without completing the period of exploration. Women were not welcome in the army, they suffered because of logistical shortcomings, but they still supported all of the units, became officers and military leaders, and were awarded medals for their valor, courage and reliability. After the war, they were socially rejected and condemned and they needed to process their identity, i.e. reject their military ethos in order to strengthen their sense of being a women. Based on Arnett’s concept one can conclude that their “in between”period of identity exploration was determined by external events and social relations.
Kamil Jezierski
Developmental Psychology, Volume 26 Issue 4, 2021, pp. 105-111
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.21.030.15486Publication date: 2021
Editor-in-Chief: Maria Kielar-Turska
Secretary: Beata Winnicka
Editor of the issue: Dorota Kubicka
Czasopismo wydawane dzięki dotacji Polskiego Stowarzyszenia Psychologii Rozwoju Człowieka
Adam Niemczyński
Developmental Psychology, Volume 26 Issue 3, 2021, pp. 11-12
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.21.016.15217Adam Niemczyński
Developmental Psychology, Volume 26 Issue 3, 2021, pp. 13-27
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.21.017.15218The paper focuses on the cognitive value of psychological knowledge about the human mind and personality development in children, adolescents and adults. It is argued that axiological engagement of developmental psychology means reference to its object of study with truth-value judgments about it. The understanding of human development entails an axiological engagement with it that is directed towards truth. Kazimierz Ajdukiewicz wrote about the significance of the anti-irrational stance in our cognition and in our actions guided by cognition toward reaching planned desirable ends within our human social world. Ajdukiewicz has pondered the epistemic value of being true or false with our knowledge; any utilitarian or pragmatic considerations or uses of it notwithstanding. It is assumed by him that one should preserve an anti-irrational stance in order to decide on the difference between true and false beliefs. It is argued that the science of psychology reduces the value of cognition to utilitarian and pragmatic values and knows next to nothing about how to identify truth and falsity of beliefs. This assertion implies an urgent need to find out how children, youth, and adults learn to deal with epistemic values of their beliefs. One should put aside the positivistic and empiricist theory of human cognition in order to open a space for a new proposal in developmental psychology. It deals with the relation of reference of beliefs of human subjects to the objects of their cognition in their social world and argues for the return to the notion of ideals in psychology in order to investigate their indispensable role in human mind and personality development.
Jolanta Miluska
Developmental Psychology, Volume 26 Issue 3, 2021, pp. 29-37
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.21.018.15219The paper refers to three issues: the effective way to reach the truth, the inconclusive and equivocal (interpretative) nature of truth, and the axiological involvement of developmental psychology. The first issue can be resolved by arguing for the need to connect a good theory with a thoughtful research program. The second thesis finds its confirmation in that the critique and reconsideration of what was previously established are the essence of scientific investigations. Finally, the issue of the role played by values in developmental psychology will be analysed on four levels: the first deals with theories and concepts of values; the second with values as the object of psychological investigations; the third points to how values enter the research process itself; and the fourth refers to how concepts and theories in psychology become filled with axiology.
Eunice Hempolińska-Nowik
Developmental Psychology, Volume 26 Issue 3, 2021, pp. 39-47
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.21.019.15220The author argues that truth is the basic value in all sciences including psychology. Respecting this value requires understanding different theories of truth, including the Aristotelian classical theory, Russell’s correspondence theory, and Tarski’s semantic theory. This is further presented and discussed in the first part of the article. In the second part, the author discusses different epistemological approaches to the correspondence between perceptual experience and reality and identifies the one favouring understanding in the science of psychology. The author further discusses and emphasises the dangers of confusing beliefs and truth in science. In the next part of the article the consequences and dangers of relativizing truth and of multiplying its subjective types are discussed. The author then refers to an approach which proposes a break with ideals in the assessment of the adequacy of assertions made by social sciences. The author outlines the difference between the concepts of idea and ideal and their roles in psychology. Finally, the author discusses and opposes irrationalism in science, outlining the importance of safeguarding the value of truth in order to achieve the goals set by psychology.
Małgorzata Steć
Developmental Psychology, Volume 26 Issue 3, 2021, pp. 49-55
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.21.020.15221The article presents the answer to a number of questions concerning the axiological involvement of developmental psychology. One of the aspects of the answer is pointing to the problem of specifying the subject of psychology without losing its essence. The issue of discussion with the dualistic vision of the world adopted by scientist-oriented psychology is also raised. The importance of autonomy and subjective freedom is emphasized in the context of trying to understand the human being as a reference point in developmental psychology.
Adam Niemczyński
Developmental Psychology, Volume 26 Issue 3, 2021, pp. 57-85
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.21.021.15222Beyond immediate references to the introductory paper, one may find in the commentaries a set of other interesting issues. They are left to another occasion, since the focus here is on remarks inviting further elaboration on the main theses’ clarification and grounding. It goes in three directions, to (i) clarify the difference between a rejection of British empiricism as a misleading theory of human cognition and a postulate to replace it with an adequate theory of cognitive reference to the truth of believes about the objects of experience in the world of human culture and civilization, (ii) dismiss from the theory of human knowledge the belief in the construction of mind-representations of the objects of cognition, and to focus instead on the relation of reference in judgments to the independent objects of knowledge within the human world, and (iii) to ground the objective status of the ideal of truth and the other values within the world of culture and civilization arrangements.
Katarzyna Markiewicz, Beata Krzywosz-Rynkiewicz
Developmental Psychology, Volume 26 Issue 3, 2021, pp. 89-94
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.21.022.15223Publication date: 2021
Editor of the issue: Dorota Czyżowska
Czasopismo wydawane dzięki dotacji Polskiego Stowarzyszenia Psychologii Rozwoju Człowieka
Andrzej Łukasik
Developmental Psychology, Volume 26 Issue 2, 2021, pp. 9-20
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.21.009.15132The life history theory (LHT) is an evolutionary theory derived from evolutionary biology. According to it, species choose a reproductive strategy that maximizes adaptation (i.e., increases the chances of surviving and producing offspring) on the basis of influences contained in the environment which impacts development in the early stages of life. LHT, due to its connections with such psychological theories as psychosocial acceleration, the evolutionary theory of socialization, or the attachment theory, is also a developmental psychology theory which points to specific development paths within an evolutionary frame. The aim of this article is to shed light on LHT from the point of view of developmental psychology – a question as yet absent from the Polish literature of the subject.
Klaudia Ołownia, Monika Wiłkość-Dębczyńska, Przemysław Zabel, Daria Kukuła, Katarzyna Zabel, Jakub J. Kałużny
Developmental Psychology, Volume 26 Issue 2, 2021, pp. 21-37
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.21.010.15133Polish society is aging. Currently, people over 65 constitute 16.5% of the population. Along with changes in the population structure, the number of patients with dementia disorders increases. Every year, worldwide, there are nearly 4.6 million new patients diagnosed with dementia, including the Alzheimer’s disease (AD). The purpose of this article is to review research studies that analyze visual-spatial deficits, eyesight and eye movement changes in normally aging people and in patients with AD. With age, various cognitive deficits appear, including visualspatial deficits of perception of complex stimuli, copying figures and visual construction, coordination and visual-spatial processing. So far, a number of studies have been conducted which show that people with AD have a thinner retinal nerve fiber layer. Macular volume and thickness reduction is also observed, which correlates with the severity of cognitive impairment. Research also points to the association of AD with eye movement disorders. Patients in this group make a greater number of errors in anti-saccade tasks as compared to healthy people. Typical for AD are delayed saccades and slow vertical saccade movement, lower accuracy and longer latency time, as well as lower saccadic inhibition as compared to normally ageing people. Identification of eye movement disorders can not only provide information on the severity of the disease but also make it possible to track its progression, and thus, probably, evaluate the effectiveness of the treatment. Further studies will verify the diagnostic value of the retinal changes as an AD marker and the eye movement disorders in determining the severity of the disease, monitoring its course and assessing the effectiveness of the treatment.
Paulina Olszak, Ewa Czerniawska
Developmental Psychology, Volume 26 Issue 2, 2021, pp. 41-58
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.21.011.15134The aim of the study was to examine whether there exists a relationship between temperamental traits and selected characteristics of autobiographical memories. 71 persons twice recalled and rated their holiday memories: in October and three months later. In addition, the participants filled in the Formal Characteristics of Behavior – Temperament Inventory, Revised Version. The results revealed that participants with high emotional reactivity recall few events and their recollections are mostly negative and non-specific. Participants with high activity recall many different events and their recollections contain strong emotions. Participants with high perseveration recall negative events first. The exploration of the relation between temperament and autobiographical memories may help discover mechanisms that contribute to the construction of given narrative schemas through which they may influence individual development.
Emilia Ilnicka, Dominika Kasprzyk, Agata Ogórek, Agnieszka Sternak, Anna Cierpka
Developmental Psychology, Volume 26 Issue 2, 2021, pp. 59-77
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.21.012.15135The article presents a qualitative study on the experience of the COVID-19 pandemic among people aged 60+, carried out in its initial stage, in Poland. Three individual, semi-structured interviews were conducted in May 2020, in order to explore how the pandemic is perceived in a cognitive, emotional and social context, and how it refers to study participants’ past experiences. Purposive sampling was used, taking into account criteria of the specific situation of people aged 60+: belonging to the group at increased risk of serious consequences of the disease and being subject to special preventive interventions. Based on the thematic analysis of the data corpus, six issues were identified: information about the coronavirus, changes in the current life, emotional and cognitive attitudes, references to the past, coping strategies and predictions. There was a large variation observed in the way of thinking about the reported events, experienced emotions and visions of the future. The pandemic phenomenon was presented on many levels, being evaluated as a difficult but perhaps valuable experience.
Olga Bąk, Sabina Barszcz, Aleksandra M. Słowińska, Wirginia Szymańska, Anna Oleszkowicz
Developmental Psychology, Volume 26 Issue 2, 2021, pp. 79-96
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.21.013.15136Learning a variety of school subjects may involve experiencing repeated inability to perform tasks effectively despite one’s efforts. The negative consequences of such experiences are called intellectual helplessness (Sędek, 1995). One of the factors relevant to its occurrence may be the feedback provided by the teacher. The main aim of the study was to determine whether there is a relationship between the frequency and type of praise and criticism formulated by Polish language teachers or mathematics teachers and intellectual helplessness in a given subject, as well as to examine gender differences in this respect. A total of 381 (193F and 188M) students aged 12–16 years participated in the study. Two methods were used: the School Communication Questionnaire (Bąk, 2016) and the Intellectual Helplessness Scale in versions for Polish language and mathematics (Sędek, 1995). The results indicate that girls and boys do not differ in the level of intellectual helplessness in both subjects. However, in the group of girls, the intensity of intellectual helplessness in mathematics increases with age. Boys experience a wider variety of ineffective feedback from the teacher than girls. The level of intellectual helplessness in students in both subjects decreases along with the increase in the praise and reduction of the often ineffective criticism.
Justyna Michałek-Kwiecień
Developmental Psychology, Volume 26 Issue 2, 2021, pp. 99-103
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.21.014.15137Grażyna Katra, Anna Cierpka, Agnieszka Sternak
Developmental Psychology, Volume 26 Issue 2, 2021, pp. 105-109
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.21.015.15138Publication date: 2021
Czasopismo wydawane dzięki dotacji Polskiego Stowarzyszenia Psychologii Rozwoju Człowieka
Anna Papińska
Developmental Psychology, Volume 26 Issue 1, 2021, pp. 9-20
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.21.001.14115The development of mental disorders in adults has its roots in the early childhood experiences. Transmitted from generation to generation, an insecure attachment impairs the ability to mentalize, which plays a special role in the processing of traumatic injuries and the healing process. The aim of the article is to show the development of mental disorders in children and adults; to present mentalization as a protective factor in the transmission of insecure attachment, as well as to indicate clinical implications.
Monika Dacka
Developmental Psychology, Volume 26 Issue 1, 2021, pp. 21-33
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.21.002.14116Adolescence tends to be perceived as a time of seeking autonomy and independence, as well as of reorganizing one’s social functioning and forming one’s identity. Young people wish for their concepts of themselves and their future lives to remain cohesive and stable. Undoubtedly, motherhood belongs to one of the most important experiences which shape a woman’s life in a certain way. It is clearly connected with the biological nature as well as with certain sociocultural expectations. Teenage motherhood tends to be perceived as a serious problem in social discussions. Becoming a mother before the coming of age is connected with the necessity of accepting responsibility for the child and it contributes to a change of the previous way of functioning. The aim of the paper is to present the specificity of the period of adolescence, teenage motherhood, difficulties and the role of support for the young mothers.
Małgorzata Tatala, Czesław Walesa
Developmental Psychology, Volume 26 Issue 1, 2021, pp. 37-48
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.21.003.14117The aim of the study was to examine the differences in religious experience in early, middle and late adulthood. The study examined 608 people who were a part of catholic religious communities. Religious Experience Scale, which includes five dimensions: Importance rating, Negative experiences, God’s support, Entrusting in God and Openness to God, was used. Statistical analyzes showed that the level of religious experience was significantly differentiated by the age of the respondents for four out of five dimensions of Religious Experience: Importance rating, God’s support, Entrusting in God and Negative experiences. People in late adulthood were characterized by the highest level of Importance rating, God’s support, Entrusting in God and the lowest level of Negative experiences compared to people in early and middle adulthood. The nature of late adulthood – i.e., the ability to discover the specificity of religious experience to understand and interpret the signs of God’s presence and action in everyday events – may account for the observed effects. The results of the study add to the knowledge on religious experience depending on the developmental stage of the person. They could be used to adjust the character of pastoral counseling to the needs of specific age groups of adult clients.
Karolina Krzysztofik, Katarzyna Mariańczyk
Developmental Psychology, Volume 26 Issue 1, 2021, pp. 49-61
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.21.004.14118In order to determine whether the reported association between the development of theory of mind (ToM) and speech comprehension in ASD children also exists at the early stages of ToM development, a group of 33 children with ASD and reduced ToM development were tested using the Belief understanding subscale of the SToMM and the Speech comprehension subscale of the IDS-P. The statistical analyses demonstrated that children with ASD presenting different levels of early ToM skills also have different levels of speech comprehension. Accordingly, supporting the development of speech comprehension can facilitate the acquisition of early ToM skills. At the same time, the results of a qualitative analysis also suggested that manipulating objects in space, moving them as well as moving themselves may help children with ASD to complete ToM tasks.
Maria Aleksandrovich, Michalina Drewniak
Developmental Psychology, Volume 26 Issue 1, 2021, pp. 63-80
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.21.005.14119Artistic activities are one of the basic and most important activities during childhood. They are a rich source of information about child’s development, their values and temperament as well as their sense of aesthetics and beauty. At the same time these abilities have attracted considerable attention as a research field of interest in developmental psychology. This article presents results of research aimed at examining the level of 2D and 3D artistic activities performed by children aged 7–9 years, as well as at comparing the quality of these works. The acquisition of additional data pertaining to the research group made it possible to conduct a precise statistical analysis, which has been combined with the observations enabled the formulation of constructive and undoubtedly interesting conclusions. Conduct of the undertaken research was possible thanks to the careful selection of appropriate qualitative and quantitative methods – i.e., the observation method, the method of analysing the child’s products, the ability test (Goodenough-Harris Draw-A-Man Test) and the statistical analysis (descriptive statistics, Spearman’s rank correlation coefficient). The artworks used in the study were created by 50 children aged 7–9 years, which formed an extensive research group in which, as assumed, an impressive number of correlations related to children’s 2D and 3D plastic skills were demonstrated. The conducted qualitative and quantitative research allowed for the statistical confirmation of the presence of both a strong positive correlation between the ability to perform 2D and 3D art work by children aged 7–9 years, and a number of complementary correlations between the age of the child and his/her artistic abilities, between education of both parents and child’s artistic abilities, as well as between mother’s education and the number of children she has.
Anna Maria Manek
Developmental Psychology, Volume 26 Issue 1, 2021, pp. 81-98
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.21.006.14120The subject of this study is the psychological analysis which uses a biographical method to analyze the last stages of a long life and the crisis “between life and death”of the two outstanding writers of the twentieth century: Maria Kuncewiczowa and Sándor Márai. The analyzed data giving insight into the life events and the personal experiences of the last few years of their lives was sourced from the last texts published by the writers. In the case of Maria Kuncewiczowa, these are: Notatki włoskie: Przezrocza (Italian Notes: Transparencies, 2010) and Listy do Jerzego (Letters to Jerzy, 1988). In the case of the Hungarian writer Sándor Márai, it will be the final part of the 5th Volume of the Diary 1977–1989, pp. 345–387 (Márai, 2020). The tasks and challenges at this stage of life, which is one immediately preceding the inevitable death, are as follows: functioning at the late old age in a changed life situation after the death of the spouse and the manner of responding to one’s own death. The analysis of experiences related to the life situation, mainly carried out with use of the conceptual apparatus of the psychology of the course of life (Bühler, 1999), allows us to observe the coherence of life events and subjective experiences in the examined period of life, which is an expression of the creative activity and the spirituality of these outstanding writers, as well as constitutes their life experience in preparation for the death.
Joanna Kantor-Martynuska
Developmental Psychology, Volume 26 Issue 1, 2021, pp. 101-108
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.21.007.14121Małgorzata Płoszaj
Developmental Psychology, Volume 26 Issue 1, 2021, pp. 109-112
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.21.008.14122Publication date: 03.2021
Czasopismo wydawane dzięki dotacji Polskiego Stowarzyszenia Psychologii Rozwoju Człowieka
Numer czasopisma został dofinansowany ze środków Ministerstwa Nauki i Szkolnictwa Wyższego na podstawie umowy nr 175/WCN/2019/1 z dnia 7.06.2019 z pomocy przyznanej w ramach programu „Wsparcie dla czasopism naukowych
Iwona Nowakowska
Developmental Psychology, Volume 25 Issue 4, 2020, pp. 15-25
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.20.024.13432Prosociality, understood as the capacity to act in a way that benefits others, is an important feature for emerging adults to fulfill their personal needs and fulfill developmental tasks. This life period is a time of exploration within various areas of life, enabling individuals to try out and choose own worldview, lifestyle, work and preferred patterns of interpersonal relationships. This review aims at bringing together the evidence on how prosociality (prosocial orientations, values, behaviors) or deficits in such features may be linked to the fulfillment of three basic human needs as conceptualized by the self-determination theory in emerging adults. The relation of prosociality to the three tasks in the context of development in emerging adulthood: (1) reaching autonomy from the family of origin, (2) achieving own identity and (3) establishing positive relationships with others, are outlined based on a literature review. Implications for future research are also provided.
Sara Filipiak, Beata Łubianka
Developmental Psychology, Volume 25 Issue 4, 2020, pp. 29-43
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.20.025.13433The aim of the present study was to analyse the personality traits and value preferences of students from integrated and non-integrated classes. Sixty-nine primary school sixth graders were surveyed (M = 12.45; SD = .58). The group of students attending integrated classes included 38 individuals. The remaining 31 students attended non-integrated classrooms. Personality traits were measured using the Picture-Based Personality Survey for Children (PBPS-C ) and value preferences were determined on the basis of the Picture-Based Value Survey for Children (PBVS-C). The results showed that youth from the integrated classes did not differ significantly from their peers from the non-integrated classes in terms of personality traits. In case of values, students from the non-integrated classes cherished values of Universalism more than their peers from the integrated classes. Correlation analyses showed that the patterns of relations between personality traits and preferred values were partially different for the two groups. Nevertheless, a similar pattern of relations was observed in both groups between Openness to Experience and values in the categories of Self-direction and Universalism.
Łukasz Tanaś, Olga Stępień
Developmental Psychology, Volume 25 Issue 4, 2020, pp. 45-62
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.20.026.13434The study explores the structure of adult (N = 170, Mage = 30) beliefs regarding the sexuality of preschool children (Ratkowska-Pasikowska, Pasikowski, 2013). Furthermore it evaluates the probable predictors of these beliefs: avoidance and anxiety dimensions of attachment, moral foundations and the levels of cognitive and affective empathy. Results show a two-factor differentiation of beliefs regarding children’s sexuality. Adults make a distinction between intrusive behaviors, related to verbal aggression or potential violation of other people’s intimacy, and behaviors combining sexuality with curiosity and role-playing. The former category is generally less accepted than the latter. At the same time, higher level of cognitive empathy and a lower level of endorsement in the sphere of purity/sanctity as a foundation of moral evaluation, is a general predictor of higher acceptance of sexual behaviors in both categories. Higher level of attachment anxiety predicts a higher acceptance of intrusive sexual behaviors, and a higher level of attachment avoidance predicts a lower acceptance of such behaviors. These results are discussed within the context of their importance for the future studies on childhood sexuality with parents as the potential informants on behavior of their children.
Kamil Jezierski
Developmental Psychology, Volume 25 Issue 4, 2020, pp. 63-76
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.20.027.13435Theories of autobiographical memory and narrative identity allow us to assume that childhood memory is represented in the mind of an adult person as a comprehensive period with its own unique characteristics that is subjected to changes during the lifespan. Based on the concept of the global coherence of life history, it was assumed that in later periods of life, childhood memories will be more elaborate and thus cognitively more accessible as well as organized temporally and logically in a more coherent story.
The use of narrative interview revealed differences between early, middle and late adulthood in the way the childhood memories are organized, as well as in the age-independent characteristics of the perceived childhood. The obtained results suggest that in late adulthood, childhood is recalled more extensively and more memories are logically organized, which is attributed to greater attention to the past resulting from the task of life summary. Regardless of the age of the respondents, less than half of the memories related logically to each other, with most of the text consisting of separate scenes not located in time. These findings raised the issue of the specificity of childhood memory and the universality of global coherence of life stories.
Maria Kielar-Turska, Anita Duplaga
Developmental Psychology, Volume 25 Issue 4, 2020, pp. 77-99
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.20.028.13436The presented research examined the relationship between the use of knowledge, the development level of higher cognitive functions and the type of education. Research was conducted on two groups of third grade students: homeschoolers and public school students (28 children in each of the groups). Students were paired by sex and factors connected to the family environment. Ability to operate knowledge was tested by The original Reading Comprehension Test and the level of development of executive functions (inhibition, working memory, cognitive flexibility and planning) was measured by a set of tasks from Inquisit computer program. Results show that homeschoolers use and utilize their knowledge better than public school students. They comprehend meaning of words more efficiently, understand cause-effect relations better and use relations of superiority-inferiority more efficiently. However, the results connected with executive functions do not show any dominance of any of groups. Ability of inhibition and planning is similar for homeschoolers and public students. Public school students have higher level of memory development whereas homeschoolers dominate in the category of flexibility. The presented results are material for a discussion on the role of the educational system in the development of late childhood students. They can also be used to consider the assessment of the value of home education.
Katarzyna Buluk, Celina Timoszyk-Tomczak
Developmental Psychology, Volume 25 Issue 4, 2020, pp. 101-110
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.20.030.13438An analysis of emotional functioning of deaf people is important for understanding their activities in different areas of life. Emotional functioning is related to emotional intelligence, which involves emotion perception and recognition as well as emotional expressiveness. The aim of the study was to compare the ability to recognize facial emotional expression among deaf and hearing people.
The present study was conducted on 80 individuals (40 deaf people and 40 hearing people). The Emotional Intelligence Scale – Faces (Matczak, Piekarska, Studniarek, 2005) and a set of photographs used by Paul Ekman in his study of basic emotions were used for the data collection.
The results obtained show that deaf people differ from hearing people in recognizing facial expressions. The analysis was conducted in terms of differences in recognition of expression of basic and complex emotions. The study included variables such as the moment of hearing loss (congenital or acquired deafness) or upbringing with deaf or hearing parents.
Maria Kielar-Turska
Developmental Psychology, Volume 25 Issue 4, 2020, pp. 113-116
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.20.031.13924Publication date: 23.12.2020
Czasopismo wydawane dzięki dotacji Polskiego Stowarzyszenia Psychologii Rozwoju Człowieka
Numer czasopisma został dofinansowany ze środków Ministerstwa Nauki i Szkolnictwa Wyższego na podstawie umowy nr 175/WCN/2019/1 z dnia 7.06.2019 z pomocy przyznanej w ramach programu „Wsparcie dla czasopism naukowych
Ewa Topolewska-Siedzik, Jan Cieciuch
Developmental Psychology, Volume 25 Issue 3, 2020, pp. 13-32
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.20.016.13153The article presents theoretical assumptions and summarizes the results of research on the Circumplex of Identity Formation Modes. The model was created as an attempt to synthesize knowledge on identity formation gathered in the stream of research initiated by James E. Marcia (1966), based on the Erik H. Erikson’s (1959) theory of psychosocial development. In the model: (1) the key concept is the identity formation mode, (2) traditional categories of exploration and commitment have been redefined and used as basic axes to distinguish eight identity formation modes, between which relationships are precisely defined in accordance with the rules of the circumplex model, (3) due to redefinition of exploration and commitment, the model can be used in research on personal identity in various developmental periods, and (4) the place of identity variables in the personality structure has been precisely defined. Circumplex of Identity Formation Modes was introduced to international literature and research on its verification and usefulness was conducted (Cieciuch, Topolewska, 2017; Topolewska, Cieciuch, 2017; Topolewska-Siedzik, Cieciuch, 2018, 2019; Topolewska-Siedzik, Cieciuch, Strus, 2019).
Angelika Kleszczewska-Albińska
Developmental Psychology, Volume 25 Issue 3, 2020, pp. 33-44
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.20.017.13154This article aims to present analysis of possible actions of kindergarten teachers and other specialists working with emotionally neglected children. The first part of the text includes definition of emotional neglect and information about the symptoms observed in emotionally neglected child. The short term and long term consequences of emotional neglect in group of children aged 3–6 were shortly described. The characteristics enabling recognition of emotional neglect and diagnostic procedures facilitating identification of a problem by teachers and kindergarten specialists were given. Basic cues concerning the work with emotionally neglected child that can be accomplished in kindergarten, including description of basic behavioral techniques, were presented. The cues for working with neglecting parent were also given. The article serves as a preliminary set of indicators for working with emotionally neglected children.
Agnieszka Lasota, Magdalena Kobylarczyk
Developmental Psychology, Volume 25 Issue 3, 2020, pp. 47-63
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.20.018.13155This study was designed to examine relationships between adolescents’empathy and aggression and parental attitudes. Two hundred and one high school students aged 16-18 completed the Polish Retrospective Parental Attitude Questionnaire, Interpersonal Reactivity Index and the Buss-Perry Aggression Questionnaire. Results showed that paternal attitudes have a great influence on the level of aggression in children, either raising (inconsistency and excessive demands) or lowering it (acceptance and autonomy). In contrast, the role played by empathy is considerably lower and only supports the relationship between parental attitudes and level of aggression. It also turned out that empathy partly plays the role of a mediator between fathers‘parental attitudes and the level of aggression in adolescents.
Joanna Szczuka, Stanisława Steuden
Developmental Psychology, Volume 25 Issue 3, 2020, pp. 65-76
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.20.019.13156The subject of the studies was to indicate whether there is a relationship between the time perception of older people and their psychological well-being. The study included 238 persons aged 60 years and older. To collect data, the following tools of psychological measurement were applied: Psychological Well-Being Scale (SDP) and The Zimbardo Time Perspective Inventory (ZPTI). It has been proven that the past positive time perspective is the one that best characterizes a given group. Numerous statistically significant relationships between particular time perspectives and psychological well-being have been demonstrated, both in their global area and in their individual dimensions.
Anna Grzesik
Developmental Psychology, Volume 25 Issue 3, 2020, pp. 77-88
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.20.020.13157Playing poker might become an addictive activity. Therefore, it belongs to risky behaviours that are willingly exhibited particularly by persons in their adolescence. In the form of a professional game, poker can also be a way to delay the moment of becoming an adult. The prevalence of poker was checked among 2054 men in two age groups: persons in their adolescence (19–24 years old) and early adulthood (25–30 years old). Contrary to the expectations, the number of poker players is higher in the group of older respondents. The level of risk of problem gambling among poker players, assessed with the CPGI questionnaire, is similar in both groups, although in case of the young adults, the number of persons showing no symptoms of problem gambling is higher. The results were discussed in the context of the available literature and in relation to the phenomena characteristic of the separated development phases.
Zofia Dołęga
Developmental Psychology, Volume 25 Issue 3, 2020, pp. 89-99
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.20.021.13158The article addresses the issue of different categories of psychological crises. The main thesis of the work formulates the belief that feeling of loneliness (consisting of three dimensions: a feeling of social, emotional and existential loneliness) in crisis states is an important moderator (predictor) of their course and ultimately psychological consequences. In the theoretical layer, the problem raised here is based on the assumptions of life-span developmental psychology theory, and in the empirical layer refers to the results of a number of research projects in which a sense of loneliness among other numerous variables has been given the status of predictor. The results of the projects show that taking the feeling of loneliness into account provides a better insight into the direct or indirect consequences of psychological crises with different characteristics. This matters not only theoretically, but also practically. On the one hand, it raises awareness of the course and psychological consequences of crises caused by various risks, losses and relational deficits. On the other hand, it advocates the use of proprietary tools to measure feelings of loneliness: (SBS-C, 8–13 years), adolescents (SBS, 12–19 years), and adults (SBS-AD and SBS-ADC) (Dołęga, 2020).
Zbigniew Tarkowski
Developmental Psychology, Volume 25 Issue 3, 2020, pp. 103-105
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.20.022.13159Sprawozdanie z 14. Międzynarodowego Sympozjum Moral Competence: Its Nature, Relevance, and Education
Małgorzata Steć
Developmental Psychology, Volume 25 Issue 3, 2020, pp. 107-111
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.20.023.13160Publication date: 2020
Editor of the issue: Maria Kielar-Turska
Czasopismo wydawane dzięki dotacji Polskiego Stowarzyszenia Psychologii Rozwoju Człowieka
Numer czasopisma został dofinansowany ze środków Ministerstwa Nauki i Szkolnictwa Wyższego na podstawie umowy nr 175/WCN/2019/1 z dnia 7.06.2019 z pomocy przyznanej w ramach programu „Wsparcie dla czasopism naukowych
Patrycja Kozera-Mikuła
Developmental Psychology, Volume 25 Issue 2, 2020, pp. 9-28
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.20.009.12264The concept of the MEANING OF LIFE operates in language as a polysemantic structure which must be seen against the background of philosophical, anthropological, theological, psychological, pedagogical, sociological and cultural concepts. The question about the MEANING OF LIFE, which includes the search for the essence and purpose of human existence, the human vocation, the reasons for making an effort to live and for why it is worth living, constitutes one of the crucial questions of various philosophical and religious systems, and attempts to answer it bear evidence to the wealth of the humanistic knowledge and reflection on human life. The concept of the MEANING OF LIFE is extremely vague in its nature, and it may also be included among the religious concepts that concern the reality which cannot be captured empirically. The author analyzes lexicographical sources, literature on the subject, resources of the National Corpus of Polish and the results of a survey carried out among Polish language speakers. The description of the concept of the MEANING OF LIFE, made by analyzing representative words and linguistic constructions that fall within it, as well as numerous psychological concepts, leads to indicating basic conceptual categories contributing to the concept today and to showing how the concept of the MEANING OF LIFE is currently expressed in Polish and in the humanities.
Karolina Appelt, Marta Jarzembowska
Developmental Psychology, Volume 25 Issue 2, 2020, pp. 31-42
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.20.010.12265The article presents the results of own research whose purpose was to answer the question whether screen time is related to the level of creative activity of preschool children. The relationship between creative activity and the possession of electronic devices by preschool children was also examined. 32 children aged 5 years and 1 month to 6 years and 11 months and their parents took part in the research conducted in 2018. The research tools used were: (1) The Test for Creative Thinking – Drawing Production (TCT-DP) Form A of Urban and Jellen (1996) in the Polish adaptation of Matczak, Jaworowska, Stańczak (2000), (2) Questionnaire for parents and teachers: How creative is the child? and (3) Questionnaire regarding the use of multimedia devices by preschool children. The obtained results indicate a relationship between screen time and the level of creative activity of preschool children. Studies show that the amount of free time spent with a multimedia device by a child after returning from kindergarten correlates negatively with the various dimensions of creative thinking. Children who more often play without using multimedia devices show greater creative ability manifested in the child’s basic forms of activity during this period: play, art and language activities.
Paulina Michalska , Anna Szymanik-Kostrzewska
Developmental Psychology, Volume 25 Issue 2, 2020, pp. 43-58
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.20.011.12266The presented study aimed at determining the school readiness of first-grade pupils by scores of standardised competence tests and the subjective assessment of mothers, checking possibilities to differentiate the results of preschoolers and pupils and correlations of both measurement methods. The Intelligence and Development Scales (IDS) and the Questionnaire of Subjective School Readiness Assessment by Parents (KGSD-R), with subscales analogous to those of IDS, were used. The three studied groups involved 36 first-grade pupils, 34 older pre-schoolers, and 34 younger pre-schoolers. The average scores of the pupils on all the IDS subscales point to their sufficient school readiness. Objective (IDS) and subjective results (KGSD-R) differentiated the children’s results into high and low, however, the correlations for the pre-schoolers were few and low, and for pupils – not statistically significant.
Dagmara Budnik-Przybylska, Dominika Kuchta
Developmental Psychology, Volume 25 Issue 2, 2020, pp. 59-71
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.20.012.12267The aim of the study is to establish whether there exists a relationship between temperamental predispositions and the use of imagery, as well as to examine temperamental traits as predictors of imagery use in children practising various sports. The study involved 82 children (boys and girls) aged 7–11, as well as 82 parents. The participants filled in the Sport Imagery Questionnaire for Children (SIQ-C), in the Polish adaptation of D. Budnik-Przybylska and K. Karasiewicz, and the EAS Temperament Questionnaire by Arnold H. Buss and Robert Plomin (1984) in the Polish adaptation of Włodzimierz Oniszczenko. The research confirmed a significant relationship between temperamental traits and the use of imagery, as well as indicated temperamental predictors of the use of imagery in sport. The article presents the detailed results of the study.
Beata Płaczkiewicz
Developmental Psychology, Volume 25 Issue 2, 2020, pp. 73-86
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.20.013.12268Gabriela Rafa, Dorota Czyżowska
Developmental Psychology, Volume 25 Issue 2, 2020, pp. 87-106
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.20.014.12269This study aims at investigating the relationship between the health locus of control and social support and life satisfaction in late adulthood. It was intended to investigate which dimension of health control dominates in the respondents and whether it is related to the frequency of medical visits, education, living alone or with relatives, subjective assessment of health as well as perceived social support and life satisfaction. It was also examined whether there are differences in the health locus of control between people with secondary and higher education. The study was conducted among 85 people aged 60 to 79: students of a Third Age University and participants of the PLinEU Senior Activity Centre. The research results indicate that it is the internal health locus of control that dominates in the respondents, which is related to the number of medical visits and living alone. There was a significant positive relationship between the internal health locus of control and subjective assessment of health. Differences between people with secondary and higher education in the health locus of control were demonstrated. There was no relationship between the health locus of control and social support and life satisfaction.
Kamil Jezierski
Developmental Psychology, Volume 25 Issue 2, 2020, pp. 109-112
Publication date: 03.2020
Czasopismo wydawane dzięki dotacji Polskiego Stowarzyszenia Psychologii Rozwoju Człowieka
Numer czasopisma został dofinansowany ze środków Ministerstwa Nauki i Szkolnictwa Wyższego na podstawie umowy nr 175/WCN/2019/1 z dnia 7.06.2019 z pomocy przyznanej w ramach programu „Wsparcie dla czasopism naukowych
Włodzisław Duch
Developmental Psychology, Volume 25 Issue 1, 2020, pp. 9-27
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.20.001.11998Psychological research has a serious problem with replicability. Several collaborative replication initiatives in cognitive and social psychology try to address such issues. Situation in developmental research is exceptionally challenging due to the difficulties in performing experiments, limitation of objective measurement methods, and small samples. The ManyBabies project is a multi-laboratory large-scale effort that gives a chance to provide more robust, replicable results in infancy research. After a brief discussion of the problems in psychological research the ManyBabies project is introduced and the MetaLab tools for statistical analysis of collaborative data at the meta-level are presented, using two examples in early language acquisition that are of particular interest to our lab: native and non-native vowel discrimination.
Danuta Borecka-Biernat
Developmental Psychology, Volume 25 Issue 1, 2020, pp. 31-48
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.20.002.11999The aim of the research was to assess the role of self-esteem and cognitive appraisal of a conflict in generating destructive and constructive strategies of coping used by adolescents in social conflict situations. The following research tools have been used in the research: the Self-Esteem Scale (SES) developed by M. Rosenberg, adapted by M. Łaguna, K. Lachowicz-Tabaczek, and I. Dzwonkowska; the Stress Appraisal Questionnaire (SAQ) (Polish: KOS – Kwestionariusz Oceny Stresu) by D. Włodarczyk and K. Wrześniewski; as well as the Questionnaire for the study of adolescents’coping strategies employed in social conflict situations (Polish: KSMK – Kwestionariusz strategii radzenia sobie młodzieży w sytuacji konfliktu społecznego) developed by D. Borecka-Biernat. The empirical research was carried out in middle schools (Polish: gimnazjum). The research involved 893 adolescents (468 girls and 425 boys) aged 13–15. Analysis of research results indicates that in the case of adolescents, lower assessment of one’s capacities and the appraisal of conflict in terms of threat or harm/loss, generally coincides with the tendency to react destructively when faced with a conflict. The research has also indicated that an adolescent with higher self-esteem, when involved in a conflict that is not assessed as threatening, implements a constructive strategy to cope with the situation.
Adam Putko, Agnieszka Michałowska, Agata Złotogórska-Suwińska
Developmental Psychology, Volume 25 Issue 1, 2020, pp. 49-67
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.20.003.12000The main objective of the study was to check for the existence of the effect of short-term exposure to fiction in the area of mentalization abilities (Kidd, Castano, 2013), taking into account the spontaneous versus explicit aspect of these abilities. An additional aim was to verify the hypothesis that at the root of this phenomenon lies the process of imaginary simulation. In the first part of the study, participants (N = 103, aged 18 to 25) were randomly assigned to one of four groups: (1) reading literary fiction, (2) listening to the same story as the first group, and at the same time imagining the events presented in it, (3) reading popular fiction, and (4) not reading any story. In the second part of the study, all participants completed mentalization tests, a test checking familiarity with fiction, and the questionnaire on practices of reading books, magazines and watching television. The study showed no significant differences between the groups in the results of mentalization tests, even under conditions conducive to imaginary simulation. Familiarity with fiction proved to be a positive predictor of spontaneous mentalization, and exposure to television was a negative predictor of explicit mentalization. The findings lead to the conclusion that short-term exposure to fiction does not lead to a significant increase in spontaneous or explicit mentalization. However, both aspects of mentalization are connected with more long-term factors that are expressed in familiarity with fiction and exposure to television.
Maja Moroch, Dorota Katarzyna Suwalska-Barancewicz
Developmental Psychology, Volume 25 Issue 1, 2020, pp. 69-84
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.20.004.12001The aim of the study was to analyze whether the individual dimensions of resilience and attachment are related to the quality of relationships with parents of people with schizophrenia and the indication of mediators determining the quality of these relationships. 50 participants took part in the study. The average age of the respondents was 49 years. Measurements of independent variables were made using Experiences in Close Relationships Revised by Chris Fraley, Kelly Brennan, and Neils Waller, in the Polish adaptation by Cezary Wiśniewski (2011); and Lifespan Individual Resilience Scale (pl) (LIRS (pl)) in the Polish adaptation of Malina, Pooley, and Harms (2016). The quality of the relationship was measured using The Quality Relationship Inventory in the Polish adaptation of Dorota Suwalska-Barancewicz, Paweł Izdebski, and Hanna Liberska (2015). The results confirmed the existence of significant, strong relationship between discrete resilience dimensions and the quality of relationship with parents. The research also confirmed that there is a significant relationship between the dimensions of resilience and the dimensions of attachment and between the dimensions of attachment and the quality of relationships with parents. It also turned out that the level of resilience is an important mediator between attachment and perceived support from parents.
Aleksandra Publicewicz, Anna Oleszkowicz
Developmental Psychology, Volume 25 Issue 1, 2020, pp. 85-99
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.20.005.12002The main problem of this study is the assessment of the relationship between the level of parentification experienced during adolescence and the intensity of identity-related senses in young adulthood. We took into account three main types of parentification: emotional, instrumental, and perceived unfairness of the role reversal experience and the identity-related senses described by Pilarska (2012, 2016). 181 people aged 19–30 participated in the study, including 95 women. The participants completed the following measures: Filial Responsibility Scale-Adult (past form) (Jurkovic, Thirkield, 1999), the Multidimensional Questionnaire of Identity (Pilarska, 2015), and a demographic survey whose main purpose was to indicate possible risk factors running in the participants’families, such as, disability of family members, chronic somatic or mental illness, addiction, chronic absence, divorce or death of a parent. Results confirmed a higher level of parentification in the risk factor group in comparison with the control group, and the lack of such differences with regard to identity-related senses. The lower level of the sense of one’s own boundaries characterized the group with the higher intensity of emotional and instrumental parentification. The lower level of the three identity-related senses (of one’s own boundaries, of having inner contents, and of self-worth) was noticeable in the group with a higher score on each parentification dimension. The latter result indicates clear negative effects of parentification associated with its perceived unfairness.
Beata Ziółkowska, Patrycja Wróbel
Developmental Psychology, Volume 25 Issue 1, 2020, pp. 101-113
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.20.006.12003The purpose of our research was to check whether young women diagnosed with polycystic ovary syndrome differ in the perception of their body image and sense of quality of life from the general population of women. A total of 80 participants were examined, divided into two equally numerous groups: with and without PCOS. Three research tools were used: a personal questionnaire, the Bodily Self Questionnaire (Scale J-C) and the WHOQOL-BREF Questionnaire. It was demonstrated that women with PCOS are characterized by a less positive body image and report a significantly lower quality of life compared to healthy women. There was a strong relationship between body image and quality of life in the study group of women suffering from polycystic ovary syndrome.
Małgorzata Cużytek
Developmental Psychology, Volume 25 Issue 1, 2020, pp. 117-120
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.20.007.12004Mirosław Michalik
Developmental Psychology, Volume 25 Issue 1, 2020, pp. 121-127
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.20.008.12027Publication date: 2019
Editor of the issue: Joanna Kossewska
Czasopismo wydawane dzięki dotacji Polskiego Stowarzyszenia Psychologii Rozwoju Człowieka
Numer czasopisma został dofinansowany ze środków Ministerstwa Nauki i Szkolnictwa Wyższego na podstawie umowy nr 175/WCN/2019/1 z dnia 7.06.2019 z pomocy przyznanej w ramach programu „Wsparcie dla czasopism naukowych
Hanna B. Cygan
Developmental Psychology, Volume 24 Issue 4, 2019, pp. 9-20
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.19.019.11725Development of self-representation is vitally important for internalizing typical behavior and for building interpersonal relationships. Taking into account the neuronal underpinnings of cognitive functions, symptoms of autism spectrum disorders (ASD) have been related to impairments of neuronal systems underlying self-perception. The present paper reviews research implementing functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) and electroencephalography (EEG) to study the neural mechanisms of self-related information processing in persons with ASD. These experiments address the psychological aspects of self-representation associated with the appraisal of one’s own traits and of the symbolic, as well as physical, representation of self in response to the perception of one’s own name and face. Furthermore, results of a study that employed fMRI to assess the perception of changeability of personal traits across time among subjects with ASD are also included. The studies under review support the hypotheses of atypical neuronal mechanisms of cognitive self-representation and anomalous perception of social feedback in persons diagnosed with ASD.
Dorota Katarzyna Suwalska-Barancewicz, Alicja Malina
Developmental Psychology, Volume 24 Issue 4, 2019, pp. 23-38
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.19.020.11726The presented research analyses the relationship between resilience, the mother–child bond, and the life satisfaction of young mothers. The assumption regarding the importance of resilience in taking on parental responsibilities was drawn from studies indicating the protective effect of individual resilience in difficult situations. It was assumed that resilience could work as a buffer between the mother–child bond and maternal life satisfaction. For measuring resilience the Lifespan Individual Resilience Scale in the adaptation of Malina, Pooley, Harms (2016) was used. The Mother–Child Bond Tool in the Bielawska-Batorowicz adaptation (2006) was used to assess the quality of relationship with the child. Satisfaction with life was evaluated using the Satisfaction with Life Scale in the adaptation of Juczyński (2009). The study participants were 340 women in early adulthood who have at least one child under the age of 2. Research indicates that resilience as well as particular aspects of the mother–child bond are important for maternal life satisfaction. It was also established that resilience plays a mediative role between the mother –child bond and the life satisfaction of the mother.
Izabela Grzankowska, Agnieszka Kruczek, Michalina Sołtys, Małgorzata Anna Basińska
Developmental Psychology, Volume 24 Issue 4, 2019, pp. 39-56
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.19.021.11727Coping flexibility is one of the crucial traits determining individual adaptive ability, which is associated with precise adjustment to environmental requirements in a stressful situation. One of the elements that assist in this adjustment is coping flexibility which relies on finding a strategy appropriate for the particular circumstances in which it is to be applied. People who are flexible in coping are characterized by sensitivity to subtle signals coming from the environment, which allow them to change their behavior and act in a more adaptive way. Flexibility in coping has not yet been studied in the Western population of children and adolescents. Due to the lack of methods that could record all signs of flexibility, it was reasonable to start work on a questionnaire that would measure this construct. To provide an equivalent to the Flexibility of Coping with Stress Questionnaire (FCSQ-14) for adults, the Flexibility of Coping with Stress Questionnaire for Adolescents (FCSQ-14A) was developed. Based on the results of pilot studies, assessment of competent judges (CVR < .6), and exploratory factor analysis (EFA) a tool containing 14 items –which examines three dimensions of flexibility: a repertoire of coping strategies, coping competence, and reflexivity –was created. The psychometric properties of the final version of the questionnaire were verified in terms of reliability and validity on a group of adolescents (N = 288), ages 14 to 20 (M = 16.50, SD = 1.58), in accordance with validation standards. The results showed that FCQ-14A is a valid and reliable method: most values of Cronbach’s αcoefficients exceed the value of .7, absolute stability is satisfactory, as is both the internal and external validity. The results of Confirmatory Factor Analysis (CFA) in a group of adolescents for a three-factor solution showed that the model is adjusted to the data on an acceptable level (RMSEA = .057; GFI = .94; AGFI = .92).
Magdalena Pożarska-Zarzecka, Piotr Oleś
Developmental Psychology, Volume 24 Issue 4, 2019, pp. 57-70
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.19.022.11728This study assesses the relationship between psychological well-being and life attitudes among older adults, while also providing a psychological analysis of life review questionnaires of 148 participants over 60 years of age (99 female). Quantitative research was conducted by means of Reker’s Life Attitudes Profile (LAP) and the Scales of Psychological Well-Being (PWB) by Ryff and Singer. The results confirmed the hypothesis that psychological well-being and life attitudes are positively correlated. Qualitative data were gathered by means of Life Review Interview (LRI). Selfnarratives of senior citizens (n = 70), both older adults (60–69) and the elderly (70–101), revealed the predominance of topics related to life values, attitudes toward life and death, and changes during the course of life. Participants who assessed their life positively in LRI obtained higher scores in Environmental Mastery and Personal Growth than participants who assessed their life negatively or ambivalently; no differences were observed in life attitudes between these two groups.
Karolina Mudło-Głagolska
Developmental Psychology, Volume 24 Issue 4, 2019, pp. 71-82
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.19.023.11729Having a passion is becoming an increasingly popular phenomenon. The aim of the study was to describe passion in subsequent human development periods and to examine whether age, sex, and education were significant factors in the process. Furthermore, it was assessed if passion results for passionate and obsessive enthusiasts (1412 people took part in the study) were differentiated by these variables. To assess harmonious passion and obsessive passion, the Passion Scale was used along with passion criteria. The development phase, gender, and education differentiated only the obsessive passion results. For the harmonious and obsessive passionate, these variables did not differentiate the result of the main dimension of passion. Gender and education differentiated the nature of passion. The most popular passions of both harmonious and obsessive nature were those associated with free time. The content and intensity of passion can be a marker of adaptation to the development tasks currently encountered by people of a given age, sex, or education. Depending on its nature, passion can be both a catalyst and an obstacle in overcoming subsequent developmental conflicts.
Marta Karbowa-Płowens
Developmental Psychology, Volume 24 Issue 4, 2019, pp. 83-95
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.19.024.11730The article presents the development of the attachment bond in romantic relationships and its main characteristics by comparison to child–caregiver attachment. The description of the developmental process begins with infant attachment styles, traces it through the adolescence period, finally accounting for how the attachment bond develops in adults engaged in romantic relationships. Similarities and differences between child and adult attachment styles are pointed out.
Beata Winnicka
Developmental Psychology, Volume 24 Issue 4, 2019, pp. 99-102
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.19.025.11731Magdalena Kosno
Developmental Psychology, Volume 24 Issue 4, 2019, pp. 105-106
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.19.026.11732Publication date: 2019
Editor of the issue: Dorota Czyżowska
Czasopismo wydawane dzięki dotacji Polskiego Stowarzyszenia Psychologii Rozwoju Człowieka
Beata Krzywosz-Rynkiewicz
Developmental Psychology, Volume 24 Issue 3, 2019, pp. 9-20
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.19.013.11291Civic participation has significant meaning for building internal resources of the individuals and is an important development task for them. In this article, which has theoretical and overview character, we present current knowledge of childhood and adolescence experience on civic behaviors in adulthood. We will deep-dive into the phenomenon of citizenship and it’s psychological perspective. Later on, we present current studies on psychological determinants of citizenship activity of young people including their personality resources, relationship with their surroundings and institutions, as well as demography and socio-economic status. At the end we focus on early experience influence on citizenship activity in adulthood. In the summary section we describe threads for future researches on citizenship of children and youngsters.
Ewa Kamińska
Developmental Psychology, Volume 24 Issue 3, 2019, pp. 21-30
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.19.014.11292The article presents two non-pharmacological therapeutic methods applied to people in late adulthood: “Montessori for Seniors” by Cameron Camp (Stroińska, 2017; Stroińska, 2018) and “Joy of learning throughout life” by Christine Mitterlechner (Dudek, 2018). The study presents a practical dimension of their application in the development and rehabilitation of the elderly, with varying degrees of cognitive difficulty. Presented views of montessorian geragogy are derived from the pedagogy of Maria Montessori, but they focus on other aspects of this method. “Montessori for Seniors” puts the focus of daily activities and appropriate adaptation of the environment to enable the elderly (also with dementia) to function independently. In “Joy of learning” the main area of issues is sensory stimulation and the proper structure of classes conducive to neurocognitive rehabilitation. Although the scope and impact of the two methods are different, they can be used complementarily in working with the elderly because their common goal, although implemented in other ways, is to support the development and learning of seniors and counteract the effects of cognitive deterioration.
Magdalena Berek
Developmental Psychology, Volume 24 Issue 3, 2019, pp. 33-47
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.19.015.11293The aim of the study was to identify factors that may participate in the relationship between the character of the bond created in childhood and the development of anhedonia in adult life.
A questionnaire survey on attachment style, sensitivity to stress, learned helplessness, level of anhedonia and perceived social support was carried out. The study was attended by 163 participants, aged between 19 and 36.
The results confirmed the hypothesized relationship between the character of bonds formed in childhood and the development of anhedonia in adulthood. The secure attachment and the perception of instrumental support, especially emotional support, are factors related to reducing the risk of developing anhedonia. The factor that conduces the development of anhedonia is, above all, attachment of the insecure character. Research shows that the character of the bond created in childhood is important for the mental health of the individual, social functioning and the possessed skills and personal resources.
Alicja Kalus, Joanna Szymańska
Developmental Psychology, Volume 24 Issue 3, 2019, pp. 49-60
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.19.016.11294Researchers of both contemporary family problems and statistical data point many important changes in family life. Among these, a significant place is occupied by the emergence of alternative forms of family life and transformation in the area of parenting (Dolińska, 2014; Golombok, 2015; Kemkes-Grottenthaler, 2003; Kwak, 2014; Mynarska 2009). Over the past years, the number of postponements or resignations from parental tasks has been increasing (CBOS, 2013b; GUS, 2016).
The purpose of this research was to explore the functioning of family of origin of spouses who did not undertake parental tasks. The study involved 210 people (105 women and 105 men) in early adulthood in legalized relationships, and the final analysis included results obtained from 170 people. The criterion group consisted of spouses who did not undertake parental tasks (76 persons) with minimum marital status of three years. The control group (94 persons) consisted of spouses of at least one child with a minimum marriage of three years.
The following research tools were used: Survey Questionnaire (own elaboration), Family Adaptability and Cohesion Scale Faces IV in Polish adaptation by Margasiński (2013). Studies have shown that spouses who do not take parental tasks assess their family of origin in a different way than spouses having children. These differences concerned the assessment of flexibility. However, the difference in the assessment of cohesion of families of origin occurred between women and men from the criterion group. The analyzes carried out showed that one and the other study group assesses their current family better than the family of origin.
Ewa Łodygowska
Developmental Psychology, Volume 24 Issue 3, 2019, pp. 61-75
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.19.017.11295The aim of the research was to verify whether persons with dyslexia differ from those without it in terms of level of achievement motivation and retrospective assessment of teachers` behavior and to define the relationship between the achievement motivation of adults and their retrospective assessment of teachers` behaviors. The study was carried out on 435 adults with (227) or without (208) diagnosed dyslexia, aged 20–30, with the use of socio-demographic survey (Survey-2) enabling to obtain information about retrospective assessment of teachers’ behaviors – 3) Inventory of Achievement Motivation (LMI) by Schuler et al. in the Polish adaptation of Klinkosz and Sękowski. Research does not draw a distinction between adults with and without dyslexia in terms of level of achievement motivation. The teachers` behaviors in terms of support, assistance and appreciation of the student were evaluated less favourably by people with dyslexia. There are significant differences between groups within the relationship between the perception of motivating teachers and self-confidence as a dimension of achievement motivation. There are significant differences between groups within the relationship between the perception of teacher appreciation and self-confidence as a dimension of achievement motivation – this relationship is statistically significant in adults with dyslexia.
Anna Paszkowska-Rogacz
Developmental Psychology, Volume 24 Issue 3, 2019, pp. 77-95
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.19.018.11296The study sought to investigate the relationship between time perspectives (TP) and the life satisfaction (LS) of young adults with the moderation effect of two dimensions of the sense of adulthood as a task and as a limitation The study involved 208 people aged 19–35 years (M = 23,45; SD = 3,17) who filled out the Sense of Adulthood Scale of Zagórska, The Satisfaction with Life Scale of Juczyński, and the Zimbardo Time Perspective Inventory. As predicted, the effect of moderation was obtained in three models, where the dependent variable was life satisfaction – in the case of a negative past perspective and deviation from the balanced time perspective and adulthood perceived as a limitation, as well as in relation to the past positive perspective and adulthood perceived as a task.
Ewa Kamińska
Developmental Psychology, Volume 24 Issue 3, 2019, pp. 99-101
Beata Winnicka
Developmental Psychology, Volume 24 Issue 3, 2019, pp. 103-105
Publication date: 2019
Editor of the issue: Dorota Kubicka
Robert Mirski
Developmental Psychology, Volume 24 Issue 2, 2019, pp. 15-28
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.19.007.10890The traditional theories of theory-of-mind development – modularist nativism, theory theory, and the two-systems theory – share a common model of mental representation. According to that model, the normative content of representation is encoded in its physical vehicle. In the present article, I point out that this claim entails the view that representation cannot emerge out of non-representational phenomena. This leads to the need of positing foundational mental content – foundationalism – and viewing cognitive development only as a reconfi guration of the innately given representations. As a result, all three models are forced to claim innate mental content, although only the modular nativists explicitly acknowledge it. Further, the idea that mental content is innate faces its own challenges: nativism does not seem to be a tenable position in either the “biological” or “psychological” sense of the term. I argue that nativism is a symptom of theoretical limitations, not a legitimate division of labor between psychology and other sciences.
Alicja Senejko, Dominika Wieland-Lenczowska, Krystyna Szałkiewicz-Bargieł
Developmental Psychology, Volume 24 Issue 2, 2019, pp. 31-42
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.19.008.10891The basic aim of our theoretical and research activity is to prepare a research procedure and a theoretical basis for such procedure directed at diagnosing three possible ways of responding to trauma (absence of adaptation, adaptation and post-traumatic development) among people who were mentally stable before experiencing trauma. In subject literature, traumatic responses are most often described by grouping into three categories: absence of adaptation, adaptation, and post-traumatic growth (development). In our case study, we introduce two examined women, who had in common the type of traumas experienced. They are women between 29 and 32 years old (young adults) whose child had died in the last 3 to 5 years. We have therefore focused not on the description of individual steps in carrying out a single case diagnosing reaction to trauma, but on the presentation of preparing procedure for such a study, i.e. the description of consecutive steps that led us from the fi rst question: what model will we use for the analysis of response to trauma and why this model and not the other one? – to the last question: what set of methods will we use to investigate the response to trauma and why this one as opposed to some other? In this case study, we present ensuing answers to the questions raised together with their rationale, dilemmas and doubts accompanying their formulation.
Wanda Zagórska, Magdalena Majewska
Developmental Psychology, Volume 24 Issue 2, 2019, pp. 43-60
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.19.009.10892Phenomenological-existential psychology assumes that the human identity consists of the subjectively experienced meanings, inscribed in the system of meanings and senses present in the world in which he or she lives. Human can defi ne and recognize his/her world of subjective meanings through narration and dialogue, and the form of self-narration refl ects the shape of his/her identity (Ricoeur, 1992; McAdams, 1993; Riessman, 2008; Gergen, Gergen, 2011). Analysis and interpretation of the story in terms of these meanings, which are manifestations of human subjectivity, seems to be the most effective when undertaken in the hermeneutic approach. The article indicates the advantages of this method, illustrating its effectiveness in relation to the analysis and interpretation of the stories of people in the senior age about life-important experiences. Self-narratives were obtained through a modifi ed version of the autobiographical-narrative interview according to the German sociologist Fritz Schütze (2007; Kaźmierska, Schütze, 2013). Content interpretation has been verifi ed by its evaluation by the subjects. The presented analysis of two out of ten self-narratives obtained allowed to discover the subjective world of meanings of the subjects, deepen the knowledge about the subjective nature of human life, as well as verify the effectiveness of the applied method.
Martyna Reder, Kinga Tucholska, Danuta Rode
Developmental Psychology, Volume 24 Issue 2, 2019, pp. 61-72
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.19.010.10893According to the Erikson’s theory, personality development is associated with a positive resolution of developmental confl icts, appearing in stages, as a result of human interaction with the social and culture world. People in late adulthood are faced with the diffi cult task of finding a balance between the syntonic and dystonic feature of ego – integrity and despair – having a chance to develop life wisdom, deciding how to perceive themselves, other people and the surrounding world. The aim of this paper is to present and discuss predictors of psychosocial integrity in the late adulthood. Psychosocial integrity was a dependent variable, while the level of basic hope, level of depression and anxiety as a trait and selected sociodemographic data – were independent variables. The study of elderly people (N = 152) used the Measures of the Psychosocial Development (MPD) by G. Hawley (polish adaptation: Z. Uchnast), the Basic Hope Inventory (BHI-12) by J. Trzebiński and M. Zięba, the Hospital Scale of Anxiety and Depression (HADS) by A.S. Zigmond and R.P. Snaith (polish adaptation: K. de Walden-Gałuszko and M. Majkowicz) and the Sociodemographic Data Sheet, designed by the authors of the study. It has been proved that the level of psychosocial integrity depends mainly on the level of basic hope, but it is also determined by the level of education of the respondents, which is important for the perceived level of anxiety and depression.
Arkadiusz Jasiński, Anna Oleszkowicz, Aleksandra M. Słowińska, Romuald Derbis
Developmental Psychology, Volume 24 Issue 2, 2019, pp. 73-86
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.19.011.10894The aim of the research was to describe the relationship between attitudes toward globalization and life satisfaction and identity processes in the group of young adults. The sample included of 403 people at the age of 19–35 (M = 22.84, SD = 2.81), 219 women (54.3%) and 184 men. The relations were measured with the use of three questionnaires: Questionnaire of Attitudes Toward Globalization (Senejko, Łoś, 2016), Questionnaire of Identity Formation by A. Oleszkowicz, A. Słowińska (referring to the Dual-Cycle Model of Identity Formation by Luyckx et al., 2006) and SWLS Questionnaire (Diener et. al., 1985). Data analysis was based on structural equation modeling. The research results indicate a lack of direct relationship between attitudes toward globalization and life satisfaction. It was established that all of the identity processes differentiate the level of life satisfaction. The most powerful predictor of life satisfaction is a refl ective exploration in depth. In the relation of attitudes toward globalization – life satisfaction, identity processes are mediators. The anxiety attitude toward globalization is in strong relationship with ruminative exploration processes. Critical attitude is in relationship with refl ective exploration in breadth and depth and identifi cation with commitment. The accepting attitude is in relationship with a refl ective exploration in breadth and depth.
Joanna Furmańska, Teresa Rzepa, Dorota Koziarska, Łucja Domańska
Developmental Psychology, Volume 24 Issue 2, 2019, pp. 87-99
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.19.012.10895The main aim of the study was to analyse the cross-reference between individualised means of experiencing a disease (assessment of its signifi cance), as determined by the level of disability and selected psychosocial factors, and the period of life of patients with Multiple Sclerosis. The following were used in the study: Disease-Related Appraisals Scale, Generalized Self-Efficacy Scale, Expanded Disability Status Scale EDSS and the original questionnaire on sociodemographic characteristics. The study showed that patients with a chronic disease show a statistically signifi cant differentiation in terms of the meaning attributed to disease as the benefi t and obstacle. Moreover, the age of the patients under study also shows differentiation with respect to the signifi cance of particular factors for the means of experiencing the disease (assessment of its signifi cance). The fi ndings obtained in the course of the present study indicate the importance of unique and typical determinants of functioning of patients with Multiple Sclerosis, as well as point to the signifi cance of the factor of age which should be taken into account in the work of interdisciplinary teams providing care to this population of patients.
Beata Winnicka
Developmental Psychology, Volume 24 Issue 2, 2019, pp. 103-107
Kamil Jezierski
Developmental Psychology, Volume 24 Issue 2, 2019, pp. 109-111
Publication date: 2019
Issue editor: Joanna Kossewska
Martyna Jackiewicz, Marta Białecka-Pikul
Developmental Psychology, Volume 24 Issue 1, 2019, pp. 9-26
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.19.006.10597The aim of the considerations is to present the usefulness of the construct of “parenting competence” according to the understanding of Teti and Huang (2004) in research on the role of parenting infl uence in child development. Through a critical analysis of defi nitions of such concepts as “parental attitude” or “parenting style,” the construct of parenting competence (PC) will be shown as allowing for a more precise, and at the same time, coherent and comprehensive characteristics of the most important components of parenting. PC will be presented as a construct which contains three important elements of parenting, namely: parental warmth, control and quality of communication. By presenting the potential usefulness of this construct, we will show that PC can be treated as a kind of social competence possessed by the parent. PC makes it possible to analyze parenting in the context of parent-child interaction, and at the same time takes into account the variability of parenting, which is culturally conditioned and, in this context, is strongly related to the developmental period, i.e., the child’s age.
Iwona Sikorska, Natalia Lipp, Katarzyna Stadtmuller, Joanna Mostowik
Developmental Psychology, Volume 24 Issue 1, 2019, pp. 29-42
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.19.001.10592Positive psychology points out the relation between experiencing the state of engagement in life, including social engagement, and the feeling of happiness and contentment in seniors. The purpose of the conducted study was to search for the relationship between the well-being of older adults and their individual life motives which were operationalized on the basis of the Japanese cultural phenomenon of ikigai.
The participants in the study were seniors from three groups: members of the Active Senior Club, students of The Third Age University and inmates of a Care and Therapy In-patient Department (N = 138). The sense of meaning of life, satisfaction with life and resiliency were measured. Additionally, the participants were asked a question concerning the value they ascribed to their own life.
The results of the study indicate that the seniors who declare they possess an individual life motive are generally more satisfi ed with their life and consider it to have more meaning. Senior citizens who are active in areas such as education or culture also present a higher level in those areas.
Anna Suchańska, Aleksandra Pilarska, Jagoda Brzeg
Developmental Psychology, Volume 24 Issue 1, 2019, pp. 43-51
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.19.002.10593The study aimed to analyze the role of attachment in the formation of chronic selfdestructive tendency. It was hypothesized that high levels of attachment anxiety and avoidance impair self-care functions and limit capacities for self-control, which may then lead to the development of an indirect self-destructive personality pattern. The study was conducted among 137 young adults, using the Chronic Self-Destructiveness Scale, Experiences in Close Relationships Scale, Self-Control Scale and Self-Care Competence Questionnaire. The obtained results showed that the relationship between anxiety about rejection and chronic self- -destructiveness was only indirect – its infl uence was primarily through self-control and partly also through self-care functions. With regard to avoidance of intimacy, the analysis revealed it had both direct and mediated effects (through self-care functions and self-control) on chronic self-destructiveness.
Andrzej Łukasik, Dominik Borawski, Anna Wołpiuk-Ochocińska
Developmental Psychology, Volume 24 Issue 1, 2019, pp. 53-70
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.19.003.10594With regard to human, life history theory postulates the existence of a continuum of reproductive life history strategies (LHS): fast strategy–slow strategy. Earlier studies show that the personality dimensions of the Big Five and worldview are correlated with LHS. In the present study we tested the hypothesis that the dimensions of the Big Five are mediators between LHS and worldview. Structural equation modeling (SEM) was used to test the model in a sample of 259 participants; additionally, the moderating role of the sex variable was included. In accordance with the hypothesis, the dimensions of the Big Five turned out to be mediators between LHS and worldview. The analysis of moderated mediation showed a signifi cant role of the sex variable in the tested model. According to the assumptions of life history theory, this indicates the role of early childhood experiences in shaping the worldview, however, the role of sex in relation to the diversity of these experiences and their impact on shaping reproductive strategies requires further research
Małgorzata Maruszczak, Elwira Brygoła
Developmental Psychology, Volume 24 Issue 1, 2019, pp. 71-83
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.19.004.10595The goal of the research was to fi nd relationship between perceived social support and psychological well-being among young people, and to verify if the growth mindset is a mediator of this relationship. The research was also aimed to examine whether there are differences between young people being reared in an orphanage and those who are not, in terms of measured variables. The study included 198 participants, aged 15 to 22 (M = 18.35; SD = 1.12), 46% females. The Interpersonal Support Evaluation List, the Psychological Well-Being Scale and the Implicit Theories Scale were used. The perceived social support positively correlates with psychological well-being. The growth mindset is not a mediator in this relationship, although it positively correlates with both variables. Young people being reared in an orphanage display a stronger growth mindset than young people not being reared in an orphanage.
Sara Filipak, Beata Łubianka
Developmental Psychology, Volume 24 Issue 1, 2019, pp. 85-96
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.19.005.10596The article presents the results of research on the locus of control in early adolescence among students from two different stages of education: primary school and junior high school, participating currently in the education system transformations. The results are a summary of the fi rst stage of a longitudinal project concerning the development of some personality variables among students. 1015 adolescents were examined with Locus of Control Questionnaire (Krasowicz-Kupis, Kurzyp-Wojnarska, 1990), in the school year 2016/2017. The results indicate an indefinite locus of control in situations of successes, failures and in generalized locus of control. The locus of control is shifted significantly more towards external control in students from junior high school, compared to primary school students and it pertains to successes, failures and generalized locus of control. Boys from primary schools are signifi cantly more internal as regards successes and generalized locus of control compared to boys from junior high schools.
Kamil Jezierski
Developmental Psychology, Volume 24 Issue 1, 2019, pp. 99-104
Ewa Kamińska
Developmental Psychology, Volume 24 Issue 1, 2019, pp. 105-106
Publication date: 18.12.2018
Issue Editor: Joanna Kossewska
Agata Ruźniak-Lubocka, Aleksandra Lewandowska-Walter, Maria Kaźmierczak
Developmental Psychology, Volume 23, Issue 4, 2018, pp. 9-23
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.18.019.9948abstract. The purpose of the study was to verify whether the birth of younger siblings is associated with an increase in the number of difficult normative and non-normative behaviors in children at the toddler’s age. The study involved 68 pairs of parents with children aged 24 to 36 months. There were two groups of respondents: 1. parents of only children (N = 36 pairs), and 2. parents with two children (N = 32 pairs). Individual variables concerning children were analyzed: the intensity of adverse behaviors and the intensity of rebellious behaviors related to the crisis of the second and third year of life. These variables were measured with the Questionnaire for Child Behavior in the Age of 1.5–5 years (CBCL) and the Behavior Scale of 2–3 years. The analysis of the obtained data has shown that the only children manifest more non-normative behaviors than their peers who have a younger sibling. Girls who played the role of an older sister showed slightly more undesirable behaviors than their peers. The results of the above studies allowed to determine that the birth of younger siblings is associated with the occurrence of more non-normative behaviors differently for girls and boys.
Danuta Borecka-Biernat
Developmental Psychology, Volume 23, Issue 4, 2018, pp. 25-40
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.18.020.9949The aim of the study was to search for the personality and family predictors of an aggressive coping strategy of adolescents in the social confl ict situation. The following tools were applied: Danuta Borecka-Biernat’s Questionnaire to Study Strategies for Coping with a Social Conflict Situation by Adolescents (KSMK), Stress Evaluation Questionnaire (KOS) by Dorota Włodarczyk and Kazimierz Wrześniewski, Morris Rosenberg’s Self-Esteem Scale (RSES), Questionnaire to Study a Sense of Control (KBPK) by Grażyna Krasowicz and Anna Kurzyp-Wojnarska, Three-Factor State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (TISCO) by Charles Spielberg and K. Wrześniewski, Parental Attitudes Scale (SPR) by Mieczysław Plopa, and Questionnaire to Investigate Strategies for Coping with a Social Confl ict Situation by Parents in the Perception of a Child (SRwSK) by Danuta Borecka-Biernat. The empirical research was carried out in the junior high schools and comprised 893 adolescent (468 girls and 425 boys) aged 13–15. The research showed that judging the confl ict as a threat, a higher evaluation of the physical characteristics of a young person, a strong belief in the infl uence of others on the positive and negative effects of the events, and the response with anger in interpersonal situations posing a threat to ‘I’ coincide with an aggressive coping strategy of adolescents in the social confl ict situation. Moreover, the analysis of the results has shown that the adolescents’ aggressive way of reacting to emotional tension, which arises in the social confl ict situation, is shaped by the parents’ inadequate educational attitude of the emotional distance towards their adolescent child, as well as by the parents’ aggressive pattern of reacting to the confl ict.
Katarzyna Markiewicz
Developmental Psychology, Volume 23, Issue 4, 2018, pp. 41-53
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.18.021.9950Procrastination studies have a very short history in Poland, even though the data reveal a significant increase of procrastinators both among the students and the general population. The results of the present study concern the interrelations between age, gender, and the tendency to procrastinate. Three educational stages (junior high school, high school, and university) were taken into account.
The study was carried out with the use of the PSS questionnaire of Steel (2010) adapted by Stępień and Cieciuch with corrections suggested by Marlena Stępień and Ewa Topolewska (2014). The highest rates of procrastination were noted in the high school students and the lowest ones in the junior high school students. Moreover, the rate of procrastination behaviors was higher in girls than in boys on both those educational levels. That tendency gets reversed only on the university level and solely in the non-adaptive aspect of procrastination.
Anna Nierzewska, Ewa Gurba
Developmental Psychology, Volume 23, Issue 4, 2018, pp. 55-69
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.18.022.9951The number of elderly people in the population is constantly increasing. The seniors’ wellbeing relies heavily not only upon the activities they undertake but also on the attitude of the society toward them. Of particular importance is the attitude of the emerging adults, because it is at this stage that the process of the identity formation takes place. The aim of the study was to discover the state of knowledge, attitudes, and the frequency and quality of contacts with elderly people among the emerging adults. 180 students (87 women and 93 men) aged 18–25 (M = 22.5) participated in the study. The following tools were applied: the author’s own questionnaire to measure the contact frequency and quality, the Kogan’s Attitudes Towards Old People, and the Palmore’s Facts on Aging Quiz. Emerging adults have proven to hold ambivalent attitudes toward old people. A signifi cant correlation has been found between the knowledge and the attitudes. The frequency of contact with elderly people was negatively correlated with the intensity of the negative attitude. The quality of contact with elderly people differentiates the emerging adults’ attitudes.
Kamil Janowicz, Lucyna Bakiera
Developmental Psychology, Volume 23, Issue 4, 2018, pp. 71-90
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.18.023.9952This paper presents a way of conceptualization of the Levinsonian Dream in relation to fatherhood and describes its shape in men at the novice phase in the following aspects: family planning, fatherhood valuing, one’s own preparation to fatherhood, taking part in a child’s life, and the relationship between fatherhood and other men activities. Research based on the author’s own survey concerning the Dream in relation to fatherhood was conducted on a sample of 33 male students aged 22–29. Qualitative and quantitative analysis has shown a substantial differentiation of men in terms of the extension, coherence, order, and realism of the Dream in relation to their future fatherhood. Rarely it constituted the main part of a holistic Dream describing the vision of their adult life, and in most cases it contained elements derived from both traditional and modern models of the father’s role.
Paweł Brudek, Tomasz Korulczyk, Grzegorz Ciuła
Developmental Psychology, Volume 23, Issue 4, 2018, pp. 91-106
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.18.024.9953Studies into the determinants of marital satisfaction are today a significant research area in psychology. The investigations conducted so far involve mainly the couples at the stages of early and middle adulthood. Relatively few studies focus on identification of the factors of marital success specific to the late adulthood, on determining the degree of similarity (fit) of spouses in terms of these factors and its impact on marital satisfaction of seniors. The purpose of this article is to identify the subject variables relevant to marital satisfaction of elderly people, both at the level of an individual and a couple. The study involved 120 participants (60 marriages) aged 60–75. We used 9 psychological methods to measure: marital satisfaction (KDM-2), life balance (KBŻ), religiousness (SRSZ), styles of coping with stress (CISS), self-esteem (SES), hope for success (KNS), location of health control (MHLC), value system (SWS), valuation crisis (KKW), and emotional intelligence (INTE). The results confirmed the complexity of the construct of marital satisfaction and allowed to identify the key predictors of happiness in marriage at old age
Krystian Macheta
Developmental Psychology, Volume 23, Issue 4, 2018, pp. 109-113
Arkadiusz Gut, Joanna Afek
Developmental Psychology, Volume 23, Issue 4, 2018, pp. 115-120
Publication date: 18.12.2018
Editor of the issue: Dorota Kubicka
Kamila Madeja-Bień, Małgorzata Gamian-Wilk, Małgorzata Lewandowska, Domnika Waszniewska
Developmental Psychology, Volume 23, Issue 3, 2018, pp. 9-20
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.18.013.9354The article presents an educational method, colloquially called ‘time out’, from the perspective of research on the phenomenon of ostracism. Described are the theoretical models and the results of research on social exclusion. It has been shown that leaving a child in isolation can have negative emotional and behavioral effects. Possible side effects of the use of the ‘time out’ method were also presented. The discussion covers the conditions in which the use of the ‘time out’ procedure may contribute to positive effects.
Ewa Łodygowska, Martyna Tyl
Developmental Psychology, Volume 23, Issue 3, 2018, pp. 23-37
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.18.014.9355The aim of the presented research was to determine whether there were connections between the dimensions of family functioning (cohesion, communication, autonomy – control, identity), behaviors of parents and the psychological syndrome of adolescent aggression (PSA). 135 adolescents, aged 16–18, were asked to complete The Family Relations Questionnaire (KRR) by Plopa and Połomski, and The Psychological Inventory of Aggression Syndrome (IPSA) by Gaś. This study has shown (1) no differences between boys and girls in the general level of PSA, (2) gender differences in the intensity of particular aggressiveness/aggression dimensions, (3) a relationship between the dimensions of family functioning and the PSA of adolescents, (4) a link between the parents` behavior in the relationship and the dimensions of the adolescents’ PSA, determined by the gender of the child and the parent.
Anna Wołpiuk-Ochocińska, Małgorzata Marmola
Developmental Psychology, Volume 23, Issue 3, 2018, pp. 39-54
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.18.015.9356The authors analyze the satisfaction with life of young people in relation to the perception of the parenting techniques. 524 school and university students from the Podkarpacie region aged 13–22 were surveyed using the Inventory of Parental Discipline Techniques (IPDT) by Gała and the Student’s Life Satisfaction Scale (SLSS) by Huebner. The dominant technique used by parents is that of introduction to principles. Fathers use the technique of indifference more often than mothers. Mothers use the technique of power assertion more frequently as their children grow in age. Parenting techniques have a significant relationship with the level of life satisfaction of the adolescents and young adults, but at different stages of development different parental techniques are associated with their life satisfaction.
Natalia Treder-Rochna, Krzysztof Jodzio
Developmental Psychology, Volume 23, Issue 3, 2018, pp. 55-64
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.18.016.9357The aim of the study was to identify the typical features of functioning of a family with the multiple sclerosis patient as perceived by the patient and his/her spouse, as well as to identify the supposed psychosocial problems experienced by the patients and their loved ones, including the children. The presented research involved 216 people (108 families) who were divided into three groups: a criterial group (CG) and two comparative groups. Family functioning was assessed using the David Olson Family Adaptability and Cohesion Evaluation Scale (FACES-IV).
In the light of the performed study particularly difficult has proved to be the situation of the adult children / grown children whose parents were suffering from MS. Their families were characterized by strong emotional bonding, over-identification, high dependence and mutual loyalty (family systems strongly intertwined with each other). Excessive concentration on the family seems to be unfavorable for children with multiple sclerosis parents. While high cohesion in certain circumstances may be adaptive, it may pose a threat to the development of the children’s autonomy and delay their entering adulthood.
Dorota Katarzyna Suwalska-Barancewicz, Alicja Malina
Developmental Psychology, Volume 23, Issue 3, 2018, pp. 65-82
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.18.017.9358The aim of the study was to investigate the quality of communication and the factors that predict good communication in the partner relationship. 118 people (59 heterosexual couples) in the early adulthood participated in the study. The average age of the subjects was 34 years. The self-esteem of the respondents and their attachment style were considered to be important factors for the quality of partner communication measured by the Plopa Marriage Communication Questionnaire. Measurements of the independent variables were made using the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale adapted by Łaguna, Dzwonkowska, and Lachowicz-Tabaczek (2008), and the Plopa Attachment Style Questionnaire.
Analyzes carried out as part of the study have confirmed to a limited extent the importance of the attachment style and the level of self-esteem for the quality of communication between partners. The study provided further empirical evidence confirming that the trustful, safe attachment is a predictor of the positive, successful, and happy dyadic relationship as opposed to the unsure bonds patterns.
Grzegorz Nowak, Krystyna Pawlas, Sylwia Duda, Aleksandra Kulik, Damian Nowak
Developmental Psychology, Volume 23, Issue 3, 2018, pp. 83-95
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.18.018.9359The aim of the study was the investigation of the generalized sense of self-efficacy, the intensification of health behaviors and the level of life satisfaction, as well as the analysis of the relationships between these variables in people with specific interests and knowledge related to health and its promotion. The study group consisted of 252 dietetics students. The following validated scales were used: Inventory of Health-Related Behaviors (IHRB), Satisfaction with Life Scale (SWLS), and Generalized Self-Efficacy Scale (GSES). In the examined group the self-efficacy was significantly higher in relation to the Polish normative values. Similary, healthrelated behaviors were significantly higher in the examined group in comparison to the normative values. However, the level of satisfaction with life was comparable to that obtained in the Polish standardization studies. Further analysis revealed in the study group a weak but significant correlation between the generalized self-efficacy, the inventory of health-related behaviors, and the satisfaction with life. Dietetics students with high GSES scores had significantly greater life satisfaction, and therefore were more content with their current life and the educational choices they had made, which translate into the health-related behaviors.
Iwona Sikorska, Barbara Sikora-Wachowicz, Marta Białecka-Pikul
Developmental Psychology, Volume 23, Issue 3, 2018, pp. 99-105
Joanna Surmacka, Małgorzata Banach
Developmental Psychology, Volume 23, Issue 3, 2018, pp. 107-111
Publication date: 31.10.2018
Issue editor: Dorota Czyżowska
Dominik Borawski
Developmental Psychology, Volume 23, Issue 2, 2018, pp. 9-20
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.18.007.8941Krystyna Pomorska, Paweł Ostaszewski
Developmental Psychology, Volume 23, Issue 2, 2018, pp. 21-33
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.18.008.8942Kamila Madeja-Bień, Małgorzata Gamian-Wilk, Łukasz Andrzejewski
Developmental Psychology, Volume 23, Issue 2, 2018, pp. 35-46
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.18.009.8943Dorota Katarzyna Suwalska-Barancewicz
Developmental Psychology, Volume 23, Issue 2, 2018, pp. 49-65
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.18.010.8944Ewa Zasępa
Developmental Psychology, Volume 23, Issue 2, 2018, pp. 67-84
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.18.011.8945Paweł Brudek
Developmental Psychology, Volume 23, Issue 2, 2018, pp. 85-98
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.18.012.8946Małgorzata Cużytek
Developmental Psychology, Volume 23, Issue 2, 2018, pp. 101-106
Agnieszka Lasota, Paweł Kurtek
Developmental Psychology, Volume 23, Issue 2, 2018, pp. 107-110
Publication date: 06.12.2017
Issue editor: Maria Kielar-Turska
Aneta Borucka-Iwańska, Zofia Dołęga
Developmental Psychology, Volume 23, Issue 1, 2018, pp. 9-23
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.18.001.8596A child’s grievance usually consists in a sudden violation of the emotional attachment and a chronic feeling of the existential threat. This may have a fundamental impact on the further development of the child. Unfortunately, the scarcity of research on the subject, both concerning the perception of the child itself and the process of grief, often involves not only the diagnostic difficulties, but also the selection of therapeutic procedures that can be discussed in terms of evidence-based programs. The subject of this article is a critical review of the theoretical positions related to child mourning, including its complex and traumatic nature.
Szymon Bręński
Developmental Psychology, Volume 23, Issue 1, 2018, pp. 27-40
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.18.002.8597When John B. Watson and Rosalie Rayner (1920) conducted their experiment upon conditioned emotional response in Little Albert, they probably didn’t realize that their study would become one of the most popular but also controversial experiments in psychology. Despite its contemporary meaning, the study was only part of a broader scientific project exploring the development of instincts and reflexes in human infants. Moreover, it surely wasn’t planned to be a final report of Watson’s research. Constant interest about the unexplained fate of Little Albert has recently resulted in two scientific historical investigations, which have interpreted his identity and developmental level in different and inconsistent ways. This paper presents outcomes of these two studies, particularly referring to the arguments on the psycho-motor development of Albert B. The results of assessment of the Little Albert’s developmental age, conducted by students as a class exercise on a course of developmental psychology, are also discussed. They might be helpful in the evaluation of different conclusions about Little Albert’s history.
Marta Białecka-Pikul, Marta Szpak, Ewa Haman, Karolina Mieszkowska
Developmental Psychology, Volume 23, Issue 1, 2018, pp. 41-68
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.18.003.8598The paper presents Test Refleksji nad Myśleniem (TRM, Reflection on Thinking Test), designed to investigate the development of reflection on thinking in children above 4 years of age. TRM is composed of nine tasks in the form of illustrated stories. The tasks assess child’s understanding of the 1st order beliefs (Unexpected Transfer Test and Deceptive Box Test), understanding of deception, ambiguity and interpretation, understanding of surprise and the 2nd order beliefs. In order to evaluate basic psychometric properties of TRM, data gathered from 259 Polish monolingual children aged 4 to 6 were analyzed. The test correctly differentiates the scores of 4-, 5- and 6-year-olds and displays appropriate measurement invariance. The TRM results are positively correlated with results of tests measuring general cognitive and linguistic abilities in children. Additionally, in accordance with our predictions, the typically developing children obtained significantly higher results than children with specific language impairment. The results confirm that TRM is a reliable and valid instrument that can be used in basic research.
Anna Antonina Nogaj
Developmental Psychology, Volume 23, Issue 1, 2018, pp. 69-88
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.18.004.8599The article focuses on the specificity of psychosocial functioning of the music and art schools students. The research presented revealed the differences between the musical and artistic talented adolescents in terms of the temperament traits and social competences. Comparative group consisted of students of general schools. 354 students took part in the study, of which 45% were music schools students, 28% were art schools students, and 27% were the students of general schools. The assumption was made that the profile of education represented by particular groups of art and music schools students is tantamount to having an elevated level of musical or artistic skills. The surveyed students of general schools declared lack of both interest in art and artistic abilities. The results of the research revealed that the students of art and music schools exhibit significantly higher level of endurance (conceived as an attribute of temperament, measured by the Temperament Questionnaire – Formal Characteristics of Behavior by Bogdan Zawadzki and Jan Strelau) than the students of general schools. On the other hand, in terms of social competences (measured by Anna Matczak’s Social Competence Questionnaire) students of art schools exhibit a significantly lower level of ability in the field of social exposure than the students of music schools and general schools.
Patrycja Curyło-Sikora, Helena Wrona-Polańska
Developmental Psychology, Volume 23, Issue 1, 2018, pp. 89-101
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.18.005.8600The article discusses the health issues of musically gifted young people, who, due to the nature of music education, are exposed to experiencing stage fright, understood as a specific type of stress that does not necessarily has negative impact on health. The purpose of the research was to answer the questions concerning the level of health of secondary school music students and its determinants. The Functional Health Model developed by Helena Wrona-Polańska (2003) was adopted as a theoretical basis, according to which health is a function of creative coping with stress.
Health and stage fright were defined operationally. In the research the standardized interview and questionnaire techniques developed by the author were used. The research group comprised 132 students of secondary music schools.
The results have shown that a quarter of students rate their health as bad, whereas more than half experience high level of stage fright and its symptoms, and cope with stage fright by focusing on emotions. Stage fright and emotional style have turned out to be negative predictors of health. The results point to the need of implementing interventions aimed at promoting the health of young people engaged in music education.
Magdalena Żurko
Developmental Psychology, Volume 23, Issue 1, 2018, pp. 103-123
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.18.006.8601Researchers into the impact of web-mediated communication on the quality of close friendship present two views: some provide results indicating positive effect (immediate beginning of a friendship, deepening relationships), while the others point to the risk of trivializing the human relationship. The correlation was investigated between the frequency of online contacts and the perceived quality of friendship in three age groups: late adolescents, emerging adults and young adults (643 subjects in total), as well as the moderating influence of the attitude towards the Internet (open / overwhelmed attitude). The research was carried out with the use of questionnaires Quality of Friendship (KJP)by Magdalena Żurko and Internet Interpersonal Relations (KIRI) by Dorota Chmielewska-Łuczak. A positive relationship occurred in the group of adolescents and the group of emerging adults. The attitude towards the Internet tuned out not to play a moderating role in the tested relationships in any of the age groups.
Aleksandra Kondrat
Developmental Psychology, Volume 23, Issue 1, 2018, pp. 127-132
Joanna Jakubowska
Developmental Psychology, Volume 23, Issue 1, 2018, pp. 133-136
Publication date: 06.12.2017
Issue editor: Joanna Kossewska
Lidia Anna Wiśniewska
Developmental Psychology, Volume 22, Issue 4, 2017, pp. 15-26
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.17.019.8065Natalia Liszewska, Aleksandra Luszczynska, Michał Liszewski, Karolina Horodyska, Anna Banik
Developmental Psychology, Volume 22, Issue 4, 2017, pp. 29-40
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.17.020.8066Mateusz Minda, Agata Piasecka, Martyna Kotyśko
Developmental Psychology, Volume 22, Issue 4, 2017, pp. 41-54
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.17.021.8067Dorota Turska, Agnieszka Pisarska
Developmental Psychology, Volume 22, Issue 4, 2017, pp. 55-73
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.17.022.8068Wybrane aspekty realizacji zadań rozwojowych a procesy tożsamościowe u kobiet we wczesnej dorosłości
Anna Oleszkowicz, Anna Misztela
Developmental Psychology, Volume 22, Issue 4, 2017, pp. 75-88
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.17.023.8069Konrad Piotrowski, Anna Izabela Brzezińska
Developmental Psychology, Volume 22, Issue 4, 2017, pp. 89-111
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.17.024.8070The present article describes the process of revision of the Polish adaptation of the popular identity measure – The Dimensions of Identity Development Scale. The article presents theoretical assumptions of the scale, reasons for undertaking the process of revision of the Polish adaptation of the DIDS, and the results of three studies in which the revised questionnaire was used. The obtained results suggest that in its current version the Polish adaptation of the DIDS is a reliable and factorially valid measure. The associations of the five identity processes measured by the DIDS (exploration in breadth, commitment making, exploration in depth, identification with commitment, ruminative exploration) with rumination, reflection, worry, shame, guilt, indecisiveness, and educational identity dimensions support the validity of the Polish adaptation of the measure.
Magdalena Debita
Developmental Psychology, Volume 22, Issue 4, 2017, pp. 115-120
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.17.025.8071Beata Winnicka
Developmental Psychology, Volume 22, Issue 4, 2017, pp. 121-126
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.17.026.8072Publication date: 13.12.2017
Issue editors: Dorota Czyżowska, Ewa Gurba
Georg Lind
Developmental Psychology, Volume 22, Issue 3, 2017, pp. 15-24
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.17.013.7573Marian Olejnik
Developmental Psychology, Volume 22, Issue 3, 2017, pp. 25-34
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.17.014.7574The subject of this paper is the concept of development in life span psychology. The claim was put forward that the picture of human development presented by contemporary developmental psychology is unsatisfactory, and that the reason for this is the incompatibility of the methodological paradigm of natural sciences adopted by psychology with the specificity of the object of study of developmental psychology. An analysis of the nature of development within Bernard Kaplan’s genetic-dramatism perspective and John R. Searle’s theory of mind and social reality was presented. Also, its methodological implications were delineated, drawing attention to the attempt of both perspectives to coordinate the logic of cause and effect with the one of intention, purposes and aims in studying human development
Ewa Gurba, Dorota Czyżowska
Developmental Psychology, Volume 22, Issue 3, 2017, pp. 37-55
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.17.015.7575Józef Maciuszek, Paulina Materna
Developmental Psychology, Volume 22, Issue 3, 2017, pp. 57-67
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.17.016.7576Małgorzata Lazar-Siekierka
Developmental Psychology, Volume 22, Issue 3, 2017, pp. 69-86
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.17.017.7577Krzysztof Dudek
Developmental Psychology, Volume 22, Issue 3, 2017, pp. 87-103
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.17.018.7578Cheating in school, which involves presentation of someone else’s work as one’s own in order to obtain a reward and avoid punishment by meeting the teacher’s expectations, seems to take place in heteronomous education, which is subordinated to a given curriculum. There is no reason for cheating in autonomous education, where the authorship of students and teachers in school is respected. Nevertheless, schools with their students and teachers do not enjoy a full autonomy and they never will. Autonomous education consists in implementing a curriculum derived from the students and teachers’ internal interpretation of educational good and giving every student an opportunity to enjoy it. The educational good has the nature of an ideal, which is not directly accessible, but which can be captured by the human mind only through inner interpretation, although never fully and never clearly enough. It is essential to introduce education in relation to the child’s development. Following Szuman (1959), Przetacznikowa (1963) and Wygotski (1971) – Niemczyński (2016a, 2017) presented a theory of the convergence of child’s development with education. It argues that the autonomous development of children and adolescents draws support from education as long as education protects it from heteronomous pressure. The more effective this protection is, the less probability there is of cheating in school and the more importance is awarded to the respect for the authorship of intellectual products at school. This could take place in a school managed in a way that respects the autonomy of education.
Celina Timoszyk-Tomczak
Developmental Psychology, Volume 22, Issue 3, 2017, pp. 107-110
Władysława Pilecka
Developmental Psychology, Volume 22, Issue 3, 2017, pp. 111-115
Małgorzata Steć
Developmental Psychology, Volume 22, Issue 3, 2017, pp. 117-121
Publication date: 28.09.2017
Issue editor: Dorota Kubicka
Lucyna Bakiera
Developmental Psychology, Volume 22, Issue 2, 2017, pp. 15-31
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.17.007.7039The lack of terminological precision in relation to a category that is widely used in social sciences, i.e. experience, provided inspiration for reviewing the scientific approach to the concept. The role of experience in the psychological development of an individual was evidenced using Aristotle’s perspective, Locke’s theory of empiricism and the conceptions of Tomaszewski, Bruner, Piaget and Tyszkowa. Different types of experience present in human life were presented. The author also proposed an original conceptualisation of experiencing and experience in relation to parenthood, pointing to factors which determine the way of experiencing motherhood and fatherhood and which are essential in the process of operationalisation of the analysed categories. The 63 identified criteria that differentiate the experiences of adult mothers and fathers evidence the research potential that arises from parenthood.
Ewa Sokołowska, Grażyna Katra, Anna Cierpka, Dorota Turska
Developmental Psychology, Volume 22, Issue 2, 2017, pp. 33-42
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.17.008.7040This article is an invitation to discuss the role and tasks of a contemporary school psychologist. The main purpose of the presented theory is to outline a school psychologist’s role as a co-creator of the environment and educational process within the school setting. The introduced M-I-P-P model consists of the following psychological activities: Monitoring-Intervention-Prevention-Promotion. The need to constantly widen the possibilities of psychological action as well as to develop the ability to create autonomous plans of implementing those actions, are part of the described model. The understanding and accurate implementation of the presented model are key to the school psychologist’s professional future, just as a clearly outlined and articulate professional identity is the necessary basis and regulator of one’s functioning in the professional environment.
Karolina Byczewska-Konieczny, Maria Kielar-Turska
Developmental Psychology, Volume 22, Issue 2, 2017, pp. 45-53
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.17.009.7041The aim of the study was to describe the syntactic abilities of elderly people taking into account possible differences between young-olds (67–75; M = 72.55, SD = 1.99) and old-olds (76–90; M = 79.88, SD = 2.92) in this area. Spontaneously produced narrations were assessed taking into account the grammatical correctness, complexity and coherence. Working memory and cognitive flexibility (visual shifting and verbal fluency) were also measured. The analysis showed that the young-olds differ from the old-olds only in the coherence of their narrations. The relation between using anaphors and working memory capacity was also proved
Alicja Malina, Dorota Katarzyna Suwalska-Barancewicz
Developmental Psychology, Volume 22, Issue 2, 2017, pp. 55-69
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.17.010.7042Anna Szymanik-Kostrzewska, Janusz Trempała
Developmental Psychology, Volume 22, Issue 2, 2017, pp. 71-85
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.17.011.7043The purpose of this study was to define mothers’ level of acceptance of beliefs and behaviours towards a child which are linked to three parental myths chosen by the authors: the myth of the supreme value of the child, of the need to ensure the child’s happiness, and of making friends with the child. Furthermore, the authors wanted to discover if parental myth acceptance is linked to rating the reasons behind the parents’ behaviour in accordance with these myths. In the study, 113 mothers (aged from 22 to 50) with at least one child of age between 1 and 7 participated. An Obsessive Mother to Child Love Questionnaire (OMCLQ) and a Modern Parent Stereotype Questionnaire (MPSQ) were used. The results of the study show that the parental myths selected by the authors are moderately accepted by the surveyed mothers, both when it comes to their beliefs and to behaviours towards the child.
Katarzyna Gołuńska, Magdalena Miotk-Mrozowska
Developmental Psychology, Volume 22, Issue 2, 2017, pp. 87-110
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.17.012.7044Referring to psychological concepts of child development inspired by the achievements of Lev S. Vygotsky (1971, 2002, 2006) and John Bowlby (2007), the authors formed a procedural model of sensitive teaching. The sensitive teaching model was referenced to experiences with mobile devices reported by families with young children. The presented study explores the meanings assigned by the caregivers to mobile devices in the parent–child–mobile device interaction. The qualitative content analysis of 97 self-narration resulted in seven categories of meaning: (1) educator, (2) amusement, (3) risk, (4) distractor and inhibitor, (5) replacement, (6) regulator of emotions, (7) biological control.
Alicja Senejko
Developmental Psychology, Volume 22, Issue 2, 2017, pp. 113-115
Publication date: 24.04.2017
Issue editor: Stefan Frydrychowicz
Maria Ledzińska
Developmental Psychology, Volume 22, Issue 1, 2017, pp. 9-25
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.17.001.6415The author presents her reflection on the issue of personal growth in times of globalization from the perspective of her own research results. An overview of the contemporary dynamics of globalization is given, with a particular focus on its driving mechanism and two most prominent aspects – the informational and the cultural ones. The effects of technological development are discussed from the standpoint offered by Herbert Marshall McLuhan’s taxonomy, particularly the effects of elimination and obsolescence.
The framework for the description is taken from the concept of information metabolism and its disorders leading to difficulties in the development of personal knowledge – resulting from both teaching and learning – conceptualized as one of the elements of personality responsible for the dynamics of development. Those disorders are viewed in the author’s own theoretical term of information stress and are linked – empirically as well – to individual correlates such as dysfunctional control.
While the author recognizes the threats to development and growth stemming from globalization, she vehemently opposes technological determinism as contradictory to the subjectivity of human being characteristic to psychological approaches. The efforts to harmonize the technological and cultural avenues of development are therefore seen as an opportunity for a new quality of personal growth, especially when attached to self-reflection.
Katarzyna Tomaszek
Developmental Psychology, Volume 22, Issue 1, 2017, pp. 29-45
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.17.002.6416The article describes selected theoretical perspectives on student school engagement. School engagement is a meta-construct including a wide spectrum of mental attitudes such as: the sense of belonging to a school, acceptance of the goals of schooling, psychological investment in an effort directed toward learning, understanding, or mastering the knowledge, skills, or crafts that academic work is intended to promote, as well as emotional involvement with school, including the student’s motivation to learn. Specific dimensions of student engagement were described as cognitive, affective and behavioral. School disengagement is a predictor of risk behavior, delinquency, proactive aggression, substance abuse, school drop-out syndrome and school burnout.
Olga Bąk
Developmental Psychology, Volume 22, Issue 1, 2017, pp. 47-58
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.17.003.6417The article presents the results of research related to feedback from parents with various parenting styles, divided according to Diana Baumrind’s conception into authoritative, authoritarian or permissive. The aim of the study was to determine what kind of positive and negative feedback is characteristic of each style. The study was conducted among adolescents, aged 16–19, N = 226. Both the parenting styles and feedback were studied from the adolescent’s perspective. The following methods were applied: Parental Authority Questionnaire (Buri, 1991) and our own method: Questionnaire of Feedback in Parent-Child Relations. The results show that the most significant correlations occurred between feedback and the authoritative style. The stronger the authoritative style of the parent was (both for mothers and fathers), the more often the adolescent experienced positive feedback (general, specific and trait-related) and constructive criticism from the parent, and the less often there was a lack of reaction from the parent. The authoritarian parenting style was connected with less effective communication, in which negative feedback predominated. There was no clear pattern between feedback and the permissive style.
Monika Topór-Pamuła, Katarzyna Schier
Developmental Psychology, Volume 22, Issue 1, 2017, pp. 59-75
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.17.004.6418The aim of this study was to examine the relationship between the body image in first-time pregnant women and the perceived relationship with the parent, the partner and the child. David Krueger’s theory on the development of body self and Murray Bowen’s theory on family systems were the theoretical background of the research problem. The participants were 144 women in the third trimester of first pregnancy. The subjects were administered a documentation sheet, the Body Esteem Scale, Dresden Body Image Inventory, Questionnaire of Relationship, and a ‘Naked Person’ Drawing Test. The application of the statistical method of data clustering enabled to distinguish three groups of pregnant women, who differed in terms of their body image. These groups obtained various results concerning the differentiation of the self and the perceived relationship with the parents, the partner and the unborn child
Michał Bebrysz, Wanda Zagórska
Developmental Psychology, Volume 22, Issue 1, 2017, pp. 77-92
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.17.005.6419The present article deals with the problem of accumulation of psychosocial resources which adults who currently undergo the depression episodes have at their disposal. Such resources as trust, autonomy, initiative, sense of industry, sense of identity, intimacy, generativity and ontic integrity are all considered as manifestations of personal integrity. The theoretical purpose of this article is to identify the resources which become weakened during an ongoing depression episode. Some hypotheses referring to intergroup differences have been formulated as far as the intensification of psychosocial resources is concerned. The research included 60 people, aged between 25 and 66, and within this group 30 subjects were undergoing the depression episode (F32.1, F32.2, F33.1, F33.2). In the research, the Questionnaire of Personal Integration by Zagórska, Migut and Jelińska (2014) was used. The Questionnaire in question is an operationalization of the authorial conception of personal integration falling within the ambit of post-Eriksonian approach. Ontic integrity (Cronbach’s α < .7) was excluded from the analysis. The presence of a depressive episode or the lack of it explained 50 percent of the dispersion of the analyzed structure of traits. The variables with the most considerable input to the differentiation of the result profiles in two groups were trust, as the axial role in the model, and generativity. The variables defining the similarities of the groups in question were the sense of identity and intimacy. The variables such as autonomy, initiative and sense of industry had little impact on differences between the groups. Defining the fields of individual restrictions and strengths might be helpful in the prophylaxis and therapy of depression.
Zofia Dołęga
Developmental Psychology, Volume 22, Issue 1, 2017, pp. 93-111
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.17.006.6420The article focuses on the problem of loneliness among school children. In our approach this phenomenon is multi-faceted. The concept involves three aspects. The first one is the sense of loneliness as social non-acceptance, neglect and social exclusion. The second is emotional loneliness, which, in turn, means a negative assessment of oneself as a partner in relationships with people of special emotional significance. It is also associated with low mood and other not very intense negative emotions. Finally, the third one is existential loneliness, which involves a feeling of disorientation, uncertainty and helplessness in the face of a perceived chronically insufficient integration with the world. The tripartite concept of loneliness was tested during the construction of a Scale of Loneliness for school children (SBS-C) based on a previously developed Scale of Loneliness (SBS) for young people (Dołęga 2013b; students aged 12–19 years). The adaptation of SBS to children was carried out in several stages involving teachers and a group of approx. 120 students of elementary school. Factor analyzes essentially confirmed the correctness of the theoretical model of the three factors of loneliness in children aged 8–13 years. The psychometric properties of SBS-C were also confirmed. The coefficients of internal consistency proved to be satisfactory: Cronbach’s alpha = .92 for the whole scale (SBS-C_TOTAL) and from .84 to .87 for the three subscales: social (SBS_SP), emotional (SBS_SE) and existential loneliness (SBS_SEG). The article discusses some of the issues concerning the construct validity, the payload, and the diagnostic criterion. A further research direction is set out.
Joanna Jankowiak
Developmental Psychology, Volume 22, Issue 1, 2017, pp. 115-116
Magdalena Kosno
Developmental Psychology, Volume 22, Issue 1, 2017, pp. 117-118
Publication date: 20.12.2016
Issue editor: Joanna Kossewska
Paweł Brudek
Developmental Psychology, Volume 21, Issue 4, 2016, pp. 9-25
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.16.019.5996Elderly people are not a homogeneous group in terms of their adaptation to aging degree. Most often, the optimal adaptation is when an elder person has a high life satisfaction, confidence in oneself and others, as well as effectively copes with tasks attributable to the period of late adulthood. There are many theories explaining the process of adaptation to old age. One of them is the concept of gerotranscendence by Lars Tornstam developed mainly in the Nordic countries. This proposal is the cognitively valuable and inspiring concept of the positive aging. It allows to esentialise the aging process and look at an elderly person in a new way. The article presents the theoretical background of the gerotranscendence theory, its main objectives and main research directions.
Anna Sukiennik
Developmental Psychology, Volume 21, Issue 4, 2016, pp. 29-42
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.16.020.5997Wioletta Radziwiłłowicz
Developmental Psychology, Volume 21, Issue 4, 2016, pp. 43-54
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.16.021.5998The biological changes in adolescence among girls are the basis for the emergence of depressive symptoms, however these symptoms can also be a consequence of a negative body self formation. The aim of the study was to analyze the relationship between experiencing their own body self (manifested in the experience of intimate relations with the men, acceptance of one’s body, attitude towards eating and body weight, disclosure of femininity), attachment styles, age ofonset of menarche, BMI, occurrence of mental illness in family and severity of depressive symptoms among women aged 18-25 years, and to determine the impact of these variables on the severity of depressive symptoms. The results show a weak relationship between age ofonset of menarche and the level of acceptance of one’s body and disclosure of femininity. Moreover, a moderate relationship exists between age ofonset of menarche and the severity of depressive symptoms, and between the strength of body self and the severity of depressive symptoms and attachment styles. Predictors of depressive symptoms are: low acceptance of one’s body, anxiety - ambivalent attachment style and the occurrence of mental illness in family.
Dorota Wieczorek, Marta Łockiewicz, Marta Bogdanowicz
Developmental Psychology, Volume 21, Issue 4, 2016, pp. 55-72
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.16.022.5999Dorota Makos, Karolina Jóźwik, Jan Cieciuch
Developmental Psychology, Volume 21, Issue 4, 2016, pp. 73-89
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.16.023.6000Previous research on shaping the value structure in childhood was based on the classical model by Schwartz (1992) and was conducted by means of the ipsative picture method developed by Döring et al. (2010). The research presented in this article was built on the developmental interpretation of Schwartz’s theory, but used a new method: Animated Best-Worst Survey for Children (ABWS-C) developed by Collins and Lee and used by Collins (2013). This measurement instrument (1) contains indicators of values in the form of several-second animations that are chosen by children as most and least preferable; (2) is based on the refined value model proposed by Schwartz et al. (2012) and (3) overcomes the problems with ipsativity (which emerged in Döring et al.’s method, 2010) by applying the best-worst scaling approach. This article presents the results of a research study on value structure in a group of children aged from 7 to 11, obtained by using the Polish adaptation of the ABWS-C. It turned out that the values from both the classical and the refined catalog form a circular structure with four higher order values and that the order of all values is as predicted by the models (with some exceptions). Finally, the consequences of the research on value structure and the significance of the obtained results are discussed.
Dorota Czyżowska, Ewa Gurba
Developmental Psychology, Volume 21, Issue 4, 2016, pp. 91-107
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.16.024.6001Building a long lasting intimate relationship is one of the basic and fundamental tassks of early adulthood (Erikson 1997, 2004; Havighurst 1981; Levinson, Darrow, Klein, 1978). Nowadays an increasing number of people are postponing their entry into relationships or are choosing to live their lives as singles. This poses the question of why young people are not forming lasting romantic relationships and the research presented here is part of a broader research which is trying to uncover the reasons why people are choosing not to enter into lasting intimate relationships and choose to remain single.
The goal of the research presented here was to identify the relationship between the degree of closeness perceived in the relationships with their parents during childhood and adolescence and the current commitment to others and experiences of intimacy experienced by young adults. We were interested in whether people living in relationships and those living alone would differ in terms of the factors identified and what precisely was the relationship between them. In addition we attempted to identify if the feelings of closeness felt towards parents and the experience of commitment and intimacy with those close to them translated into an assessment of its quality and whether there were differences depending on gender.
431 individuals participated in the research aged between 23 and 37 (M= 29,65 ; SD= 4,29 ). 217 women were studied (M=29,62; SD=4,35) and 214 men (M= 29,67 ; SD= 4,24). Those in relationships constituted 46% , and those living alone numbered 54 % of the group studied. The analysis indicated a relationship between the degree of closeness with parents and the feeling of intimacy with one’s partner which in turn had an impact on the perceived quality of the relationship. A relation was also observed between closeness to parents, intimacy and commitment to those closest to the subjects.
Ewa Czerwińska-Jakimiuk
Developmental Psychology, Volume 21, Issue 4, 2016, pp. 111-114
Olga Mironiuk-Gracz, Karolina Byczewska-Konieczny
Developmental Psychology, Volume 21, Issue 4, 2016, pp. 115-122
Publication date: 14.11.2016
Issue Editor: Maria Kielar-Turska
Henryk Gasiul
Developmental Psychology, Volume 21, Issue 3, 2016, pp. 9-24
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.16.013.5524The author presents selected dominant contemporary approaches to the interpretation of subjectivity. Subjectivity is essential to understanding individual development. Unfortunately, one has to do with a huge amount of different interpretations. The article presents ways of interpreting subjectivity from the agentive internalistic and agentive externalistic positions, as well as proposes considering them from a personalistic perspective. The personalistic perspective of interpreting subjectivity is but one among other perspectives.
Czesław Walesa
Developmental Psychology, Volume 21, Issue 3, 2016, pp. 25-33
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.16.014.5525Gravity is a natural force found in different dimensions. Mental gravity includes inertia, addiction, mindlessness and other similar processes or states. Anti-gravity, on the other hand, includes activity, freedom, openness, mindfulness, etc. Religiosity is a personal and positive relation of human to God. Processes of religiosity – according to its structural definition – include the following clusters of processes (parameters): (1) religious awareness, (2) religious feelings, (3) religious decisions (preferences), (4) a bond with a community of believers, (5) religious practices, (6) religious morality, (7) religious experiences, and (8) forms of confessing the faith (especially prayerful and sacrificial acts and the sense of mystery). The article is meant as an analysis of anti-gravitational processes of religiosity in the abovementioned domains. It presents a specific language about and a fragment of the language of religiosity.
Małgorzata Steć
Developmental Psychology, Volume 21, Issue 3, 2016, pp. 35-46
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.16.015.5526This article aims to present a review of the essence of and educational opportunities offered by the Konstanz Method of Dilemma-Discussion (KMDD®) by Prof. Georg Lind from the University of Konstanz. The article presents the theoretical background and methodological aspects of KMDD®, as well as its importance in processes of supporting the socio-moral and democratic development. Ways of evaluating the results of applying the method are also discussed, as well as international studies supporting its effectiveness in educational practice. KMDD® is well recognized and successfully used in more than forty countries around the world, where it supports the moral development of young people and adults by building their moral and democratic competence.
Błażej Smykowski
Developmental Psychology, Volume 21, Issue 3, 2016, pp. 47-66
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.16.016.5527The way a given theoretical concept is interpreted depends on several temporal perspectives. First of all, not only is it the age of the interpreter and the historical time when the interpretation takes place, but also the chronology of successive versions of the author’s ideas or their interpretations by others. Without taking those into account, the misinterpretation which could have been avoided may be mistaken with another which is impossible to avoid. Thus, a correct interpretation might be considered wrong. Allowing for time variables must result in the conclusion that a close-to-correct interpretation of a theoretical concept is available only to a mature person who has both the physical and psychological access to the content of the theory, and who has become independent from the historical actuality to a large extent.
The root of this reflection is in the book Versus. About the structural duality of development phases in the ecology of life cycle in Erik H. Erikson’s psychodynamic model published by Lech Witkowski in 2015. The concept presented in the work seems a perfect example of such a dependency. Only at the last phase of his lifecycle did the author himself fully understand and synthetically describe the logic of the stage development of the ego and self identity, as well as the central content and dynamics of inner- and interstage processes.
Anna Kołodziejczyk
Developmental Psychology, Volume 21, Issue 3, 2016, pp. 69-88
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.16.017.5528Research suggests that social cognitive abilities, particularly social perspective-taking and theory of mind (ToM), play a role in the development of persuasion in early and middle childhood. In the presented study, the inner state talk and perspective-taking in a natural persuasive play context were studied to explore the relational specificity of persuading an adult or a peer. Ninety 5 to 7-year-olds participated in a novel persuasion role-play task in which children were invited to convince an interactive puppet-partner. The results showed that while persuading an adult, children more often included terms used to denote thoughts, memories, or knowledge, that is cognitive inner state talk, as compared to persuading a peer playmate, when they more often referred to emotional and motivational states. Talking about the personal perspective of the persuaded person or the shared perspective of both play-characters was connected with the use of direct-bilateral persuasive strategies, like compromise or bargaining.
Kamil Olszewski, Elżbieta Talik, Maria Oleś
Developmental Psychology, Volume 21, Issue 3, 2016, pp. 89-103
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.16.018.5529The aim of this study is to identify the relationship between the tendency to worry and the perception of parental attitudes in early adolescence.
A group of 103 children, aged from 13 to 15, was examined. The participants completed two tests: Anne Roe’s Parent-Child Relations Questionnaire and Bruce Chorpita’s Penn State Worry Questionnaire for Children.
Adolescents, girls more often than boys, worry at a moderate level. The results confirm that the intensity level of worry is associated with the perception of some parental attitudes: the more young people perceive their parents as demanding and rejecting, the more they worry. The tendency to worry is mostly associated with the rejecting mother’s attitude.
Joanna Kossewska
Developmental Psychology, Volume 21, Issue 3, 2016, pp. 107-113
Kamil Jezierski
Developmental Psychology, Volume 21, Issue 3, 2016, pp. 115-119
Publication date: 23.07.2016
Issue Editor: Dorota Kubicka
Maria Straś-Romanowska
Developmental Psychology, Volume 21, Issue 2, 2016, pp. 15-25
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.16.007.5085Drawing on anthropological philosophy, the article describes the personal subjectas well as forms of manifesting the attributes of subjectivity by a typical person who lives in contemporary individual and liberal culture. The main attributes of the personal subject are: mental openness, with special emphasis on reflective consciousness, spiritual sensitivity, free will and transgression. It was observed that in the context of cultural changes the personal attributes sometimes adopt the form of their opposites, which manifests itself as a new/another constraint, avoiding reflection or actions motivated first and foremost by pragmatic factors.
Magdalena Jančina
Developmental Psychology, Volume 21, Issue 2, 2016, pp. 27-40
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.16.008.5086The article is a continuation of theoretical deliberations on self-creation presented in the earlier work (Jančina, Kubicka, 2014). It contains a detailed characteristics of this phenomenon, assuming that the object of self-creation activities is the Self, in the sense of its cognitive representation named the Self, and more precisely – the identity of a person. I suggest introducing the concept of a complex self-creation competence, which consists of such detailed competencies as autobiographical knowledge (thinking), autonomy, creativity, transgression, defining and committing oneself to fulfilling long-term goals, and self-reflection. In addition, I search for an adequate method for researching these detailed competencies. The article also contains argumentation in favour of the analysis of the content of self-narratives as the most accurate method of self-creation research as defined here. Therefore, McAdams’s life story model is presented, and a strong argument in favour of such a choice of the research method is given in the form of the deliberations of Zbigniew Pietrasiński, the author of the self-creation concept itself, who conducted research by analysing the biographies of well-known historical figures.
Dorota Katarzyna Suwalska-Barancewicz
Developmental Psychology, Volume 21, Issue 2, 2016, pp. 41-52
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.16.009.5087The attachment theory is one of the more significant and empirically confirmed theories, which provides a relatively full explanation of the specificity of close relationships. Continuators of Bowlby (2007), while making a significant contribution to the development of research in this area, contributed to the knowledge of how people with variously shaped attachment styles function.
In the presented study we analyze relationships of attachment, with such variables as: emotional control, communication, preference as regards the partner, perception of the partner, as well as aggression and violence towards the partner.
This article presents empirical evidence that safely, confidently attached people (whose avoidance and anxiety level is low), have the ability to function in relationships, increasing the chance of success in a relationship and the sense of contentment of the partners. In contrast, individuals with insecure attachment patterns may be more likely to experience difficulty in a close relationship
Paulina Michalska , Anna Szymanik-Kostrzewska, Ewa Gurba, Janusz Trempała
Developmental Psychology, Volume 21, Issue 2, 2016, pp. 55-71
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.16.010.5088The article presents the results of two studies concerning the development of postformal reasoning as proposed by G. Lavouvie-Vief, with the use of the everyday problem solving dilemmas taken from the research conducted by R.A. Sebby and D.R. Papini in the Polish adaptation by E. Gurba. Study no. 1 tested the levels of reasoning in 3 age groups: early, middle, and late adulthood, providing the participants with a choice of readymade answers. Study no. 2 tested the levels of reasoning amongst the participants while using a modified version of the test which required giving an answer to an open question pertaining to the solution of a dilemma. Regardless of their age, the participants revealed a lower level of postformal reasoning in Study no. 2. Differences in the influence of spurious variables (sex, level of education) on the presented level of reasoning in the respective Studies were also observed. The discussion attempts to explain the observed differences with relation to the cognitive requirements of both methods of study as well as the form and content of the applied dilemmas
Jan Cieciuch , Karolina Toczyłowska-Niemiec, Claudio Barbaranelli
Developmental Psychology, Volume 21, Issue 2, 2016, pp. 73-85
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.16.011.5089The article presents the psychometric properties of the Polish version of the Big Five Questionnaire – Children (BFQ-C; Barbaranelli, Caprara, Rabasca & Pastorelli, 2003). BFQ-C is a questionnaire measuring five personality traits (energy/extraversion, agreeableness, conscientiousness, emotional instability and intellect/openness) in late childhood and adolescence. Two versions of BFQ-C were used in the study: in the first one children and adolescents describe themselves, and in the second one other informants (parents and teachers) provide information about children and adolescents. The participants in the study were 1,241 children and adolescents (aged from 7 to 15 years), 226 parents, and 475 teachers. The reliability was assessed by Cronbach’s alpha. The structural validity was assessed in a set of factor analyses. Convergent and discriminant validity were assessed using a multitrait–multimethod matrix (five traits and two versions of BFQ-C) and analyzed in structural equation modeling. The external validity was assessed by inspecting the relations between the personality traits measured by BFQ-C and temperamental traits in the model proposed by Oakland, Glutting, and Horton (1996), as well as temperamental traits in the model by Buss and Plomin (Oniszczenko, 1997). Both reliability and all kinds of validity were at acceptable levels, which leads to the conclusion that BFQ-C can be used in scientific research.
Magdalena Przedniczek
Developmental Psychology, Volume 21, Issue 2, 2016, pp. 87-100
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.16.012.5090The aim of this study is to compare the effectiveness of working memory and attention training and sports training in improving intellectual functioning in children of school age. The participants of the study were pupils aged 10–12 (N=51). The DMI-2 Test in the group version for older kids (S) was used for diagnosis in pre- and posttest. Two groups participated in a cognitive training of two types: 1) a training based on computer cognitive tasks and mnemonics, 2) LEGO Mindstorms building and programming classes. The third group participated in sport (football) training. The training programmes lasted six weeks and included two 90-minute meetings per week. In the period between the pre- and posttest, all three training groups improved in terms of reasoning by analogy and refilling the system consisting of two similar pairs (class). However, only two groups: (1) using the computer cognitive tasks and (2) using LEGO bricks achieved significant changes in DMI-2 tasks involving completing 4-component systems (relations). The results can be used in designing training programmes for students with learning difficulties.
Magdalena Kosno
Developmental Psychology, Volume 21, Issue 2, 2016, pp. 103-105
Anna Rybka, Aneta Garncarz
Developmental Psychology, Volume 21, Issue 2, 2016, pp. 107-113
Publication date: 31.03.2016
Issue Editor: Joanna Kossewska
Justyna Kotowicz
Developmental Psychology, Volume 21, Issue 1, 2016, pp. 9-18
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.16.001.4790The sign language contains manual signs and non-manual components which be can used as markers for certain grammatical structures. This article presents the process of acquisition of non-manual sign language elements in children whose first of communication is the sign language. This analysis includes grammatical structures that are signalled non-manually: negation, adverbial, wh-question, closed-ended question, conditional clause and topicalisation (emphasis on the sentence topic). We also present a discussion about the mechanisms of acquiring non-manual elements of the sign language.
Marzanna Farnicka, Hanna Liberska, Jari Erik Nurmi
Developmental Psychology, Volume 21, Issue 1, 2016, pp. 21-32
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.16.002.4791The article presents the results of research on chosen symptoms and situational determinants of the “success at school” of a primary school pupil. It was assumed that for a child to adapt to the challenges posed by school the following factors were important: a sense of acceptance, a sense of support, a sense of social exclusion and the ability to assess one’s own achievements. The study involved 173 children from grades 2 to 5. Data were collected twice using three tools: SPAS questionnaire (Matějček, Vágnerová, 1992), SUSPO (Mikšík, 2004) and QSL authoring tool. The results show: 1) the role of subjective conditions and their changes over time, 2) the role of the climate in educational environment, and 3) the prospect for the optimization of and increase in the effectiveness with which children adapt to school in the first period of learning
Ludwika Wojciechowska, Magdalena Jasik
Developmental Psychology, Volume 21, Issue 1, 2016, pp. 33-47
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.16.003.4792The objective of this study was to answer the question whether the properties of parental attitudes manifested by mothers and fathers are related to the level of adolescent shyness and the intensity of egocentrism in their offspring. The question of the relationship between shyness and egocentrism of adolescents was also posed. The following findings were expected: 1) the overprotective, overdemanding, inconsistent and rejecting attitudes are to a greater degree connected with a higher level of adolescent shyness than the accepting and autonomy giving attitudes; 2) the overprotective, overdemanding, inconsistent and rejecting attitudes are to a greater degree connected with a higher level of egocentrism in adolescents than the accepting and autonomy giving attitudes, and: 3) there exists a positive relationship between shyness and the level of adolescent egocentrism.
The theoretical grounds for this research were: the concept of parental attitudes (Plopa, 2005), the concept of adolescent egocentrism (Elkind, 1967, 1985) taking two types of egocentrism into account – the personal fable and imaginary audience, as well as the concept of shyness as developed by Philip Zimbardo (2011).
A total of 120 Polish “gymnasium” (lower secondary school) pupils aged 13–14 years were tested. The shy group amounted to 84 persons, 43 of whom were girls and 41 were boys. The absence of shyness was declared by 36 persons, 20 girls and 16 boys. Three measuring instruments were used: The Parental Attitude Scale of Plopa in the version for mothers and fathers, the Stanford Shyness Survey of Zimbardo and the unpublished Adolescent Egocentrism Questionnaire of Ludwika Wojciechowska and Żywek. The results of the research allowed the following statements to be made: 1) the attitude of excessive demands on the part of the mother is most strongly connected with adolescent shyness; 2) the overprotective attitude manifested by the mother most strongly relates to the egocentrism dimension of the personal fable; 3) the mother’s attitude of excessive demands is most strongly related to the egocentrism dimension of the imaginary audience; 4) a positive relationship exists between shyness and the level of adolescent egocentrism.
Katarzyna Lubiewska, Karolina Głogowska, Kinga Mickiewicz, Ewa Wojtynkiewicz, Paweł Izdebski, Cezary Wiśniewski
Developmental Psychology, Volume 21, Issue 1, 2016, pp. 49-63
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.16.004.4793The Experiences in Close Relationships-Revised is one of the most popular self-report instruments assessing adult attachment in the sphere of avoidance and anxiety. Although the scale is used in Poland, its psychometric properties are not sufficiently documented. In the study, we set out to: estimate the psychometric properties of the scale, controlling for the common method variance (method bias); and introduce a short version of the scale. The sample was composed of 781 adults between the age of 16 and 81. The results of the confirmatory factor analysis supported two-factorial ECR-R structure indicating problems with low factor loadings, which deteriorated even more after controlling for the common method variance. On the basis of the content of the items and their psychometric properties we proposed a short 16-item form of the ECR-R scale which revealed a good model fit and factor loadings. However, the effects of the common method variance on factor loadings and the avoidance-anxiety relation were found to be substantial. The results revealed that the relation between avoidance and anxiety was due to the shared method variance and not to the attachment itself. The issues concerning the construction of attachment self-report scales are discussed along with practical implications for the further use of the ECR-R scale in Poland, named as the DBZ-R (36-items) or the DBZ-RS (the short version).
Dariusz Krok
Developmental Psychology, Volume 21, Issue 1, 2016, pp. 65-76
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.16.005.4794The religiousness of young people is strongly associated with discovering life goals, identity formation, and the sphere of values. During this period there occur dynamic changes in religious beliefs and behaviours, as well as the process of discovering the meaning of life and creating hope. The purpose of this article is to examine the relationships between religion analysed in terms of personal constructs and post-critical beliefs and the sense of meaning in life, as well as two types of hope: basic hope and hope for success. The research was conducted on a group of 198 people (104 men and 94 women) aged 18–23 years. The results showed that religiousness is more strongly associated with the presence of meaning in life than with searching for it, and more with basic hope than with hope for success. The factor that differentiated people in terms of meaning in life and hope was the character of their attitude towards religion.
Dariusz Kurzydło, Wanda Zagórska
Developmental Psychology, Volume 21, Issue 1, 2016, pp. 77-96
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.16.006.4795Referring to both the psychological concepts of human development (Erikson, 1997; Labouvie-Vief, 1990) and its anthropological ones (Eliade, 1990; van Gennep, 2006; Turner, 2010), and embedding them in the psychology of myth (Pankalla, Klaus, 2010; Zagórska, 2007), the authors conducted an empirical exploration aimed at finding out whether the adolescents’ behaviours which serve to leave childhood bear the markings of contemporary rites of passage (in their initial phase). However, because these behaviours are highly degraded, residual, they were defined as quasi-rituals of leaving childhood, and in a broader sense – quasi-mythical behaviours (mythos-type behaviours). Analyzing the narratives of 40 teenagers aged 13–16, of both sexes, from a few lower secondary schools in a small and a big city and using the Interpretative Phenomenological Analysis (IPA), a number of this kind of behaviours were identified. It was found that teenagers – in the absence of socially sanctioned rites of passage and clear criteria of being adult – imitate some adult behaviours, often negative ones, hoping thereby to obtain the status of an adult. Quasi-rituals of leaving childhood are for adolescents a concrete expression of the activity of separating themselves from being perceived as children, but also from any attempts on the part of adults at imposing tasks of adulthood on them. The mythical overtones of such activities stem from the teenagers’ belief in the uniqueness of the adult phase of life, to which they ascribe divine qualities.
Magdalena Debita
Developmental Psychology, Volume 21, Issue 1, 2016, pp. 99-102
Olga Mironiuk-Gracz, Karolina Byczewska-Konieczny
Developmental Psychology, Volume 21, Issue 1, 2016, pp. 103-107
Publication date: 21.12.2015
Issue editor: Dorota Czyżowska
Beata Krzywosz-Rynkiewicz, Anna M. Zalewska
Developmental Psychology, Volume 20, Issue 4, 2015, pp. 11-23
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.15.019.4462In the article the essence and range of young people citizenship is discussed. Results of research on citizenship activity of Poles in relation to other young Europeans and to adolescence stages are presented. Referring to citizenship model by Zalewska and Krzywosz-Rynkiewicz (2011) 3796 adolescents aged 11, 14, 18 from 11 European countries including 361 Poles were examined with Citizenship Behavior Questionnaire. Young Poles in relation to other European adolescents have presented higher level of personal and social citizenship, of national identity and patriotism, but higher reluctance to politics and protests. Their involvement in personal and for change activity has been stable though their engagement into other forms of citizenship has decreased with age.
Hanna Liberska, Marzanna Farnicka
Developmental Psychology, Volume 20, Issue 4, 2015, pp. 25-44
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.15.020.4463The present paper verifies the assumptions about the importance of parental attitudes for the mechanism of formation of aggressive behavior in adolescents. It was carried out on the basis of a series of three studies. The aim was to assess the significance of any links between: parental attitudes and expression of aggressive behavior of the respondents in the second phase of adolescence (N = 237). The basic research question concerned the problem of stability and change during aggression in relation to the gender of respondents and their parenting attitudes. The study used: Parental’ Attitude Scale (SPR-2) (Plopa, 1987, 2005), tools for the study of aggression (Wójcik) and BPAQ (Buss, Perry, 1992). The research found that there are: (1) significant differences between girls and boys in their late adolescence in the manifestations of aggression, (2) a significant relationship between specific parental attitudes and adolescent aggressive behavior depending on the gender of children a,nd their parents’, (3) changes in the manifestation of aggressiveness depending on the cultural context. Constellations of parental attitudes have diagnosed, which substantially relate to the intensity of adolescent aggressive responses and the occurrence of physical forms of aggression. It was also found that the influence of parental attitudes on growing children’s aggressive behavior is limited by gender of the child and the parent.
Eufrozyna Gruszecka
Developmental Psychology, Volume 20, Issue 4, 2015, pp. 45-57
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.15.021.4464The goal of the study was to investigate the link between adolescents’ home environment and their sense of injustice, emotional vulnerability and willingness to forgive others. Participants of the study were junior high school students raised in functional and dysfunctional families as well as students brought up in children’s homes. All participants filled in a detailed personal inventory which measured the degree of their sense of injustice. In the second part of the study we asked participants to predict their reactions to hypothetical scenarios. Participants from dysfunctional families cited a greater variety of sources of their sense of injustice and reported experiencing a sense of injustice more often than their peers, whereas participants who lived in children’s homes more often than other participants felt that fate had dealt them short. Willingness to forgive others was greatest among participants brought up in children’s homes, the remaining two groups of participants did not differ from each other in this respect.
Dorota Czyżowska, Ewa Gurba
Developmental Psychology, Volume 20, Issue 4, 2015, pp. 59-71
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.15.022.4465The aim of this study was to analyze the relationship between sense of coherence, identity styles and religiosity during emerging adulthood. The study included 153 subjects (aged 18 to 25 years). The hypothesis about relationship between identity styles and sense of coherence was only partially confirmed. The analysis showed that a diffuse – avoidant style reduce sense of coherence as well as its components: comprehensibility and meaningfulness. The other identity styles were not predictors of sense of coherence but there was a positive correlation between normative style and sense of coherence. There was no relationship between informational style and sense of coherence. Also a level of religiosity proved to have no relevance to sense of coherence.
Aleksandra Pilarska
Developmental Psychology, Volume 20, Issue 4, 2015, pp. 73-90
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.15.023.4466The article presents the results of a study carried out in a group of 80 women in early adulthood and 72 women in later adulthood. The primary goals were to examine the relationships of need for cognition and two forms of cognitive processing of self-related information (reflective self-focus and integrative self-knowledge) to multiple dimensions of sense of identity, and to test for life stage differences in the magnitude of these relationships. Significant differences were observed in the reflective self-focus levels and strength of sense of identity between the two groups of women. The obtained results also indicated that – regardless the stage of life – a tendency to engage in and enjoy effortful cognitive tasks as well as a tendency to integrate past, present, and desired future self-experience into a meaningful whole favored maintenance of sense of identity. The effects of reflection appeared more complicated.
Alicja Senejko, Zbigniew Łoś
Developmental Psychology, Volume 20, Issue 4, 2015, pp. 91-104
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.15.024.4467This article contains the main assumptions of the social-cognitive model of identity construction by M. Berzonsky, the characteristics of the Polish adaptation of the Identity Style Inventory (ISI-5), a report from five studies conducted using this questionnaire, and an evaluation of the ISI-5’s Polish adaptation. We followed the guidelines provided by the authors of the ISI-5 (Berzonsky, Soenens, Luyckx, Smits, Papini, Goossens, 2013), adjusting our procedure and analysis of results to the original ISI-5 study. The results obtained showed the adaptation to have similar properties to its original. Confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) revealed the fit of the Polish data to the three-factor model of identity styles (RMSEA = .062; sRMR = .069; GFI = .90). Cronbach’s alpha reliability coefficients for ISI-5 scales were satisfactory and measured .68 for the Normative style scale, .77 for the Informational style scale, .71 for the Diffuse-avoidant style scale, and .80 for the Commitment scale. Intercorrelations between ISI-5 scales were also similar to those provided by Berzonsky et al. (2013). Correlations of ISI-5 scales with dimensions of identity (DIDS) and factors of personality (NEO-FFI) were approximate to the expected ones as well
Katarzyna Stawiarska
Developmental Psychology, Volume 20, Issue 4, 2015, pp. 107-116
Wanda Zagórska, Jan Cieciuch
Developmental Psychology, Volume 20, Issue 4, 2015, pp. 117-121
Publication date: 03.11.2015
Issue editor: Stefan Frydrychowicz
Anna Szymanik-Kostrzewska
Developmental Psychology, Volume 20, Issue 3, 2015, pp. 11-22
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.15.013.3802The article focuses on a a social phenomenon known as the age compression of behaviours, described as the dysregulation of boundaries between childhood and adulthood in the area of consumer, social and product usage behaviours. The age compression of behaviours was defined as reducing the time from the birth of the child to the moment of undertaking behaviours, which in previous decades were common for adolescents and adults. This phenomenon is discussed from a cognitive perspective, as an effect of acceleration related to certain areas of both children’s cognitive development, as and socio-cultural changes which influence changes in the way children are treated. It was assumed that we simultaneously observe behaviours which, on the one hand, are based on a dysregulation of cognitive processes – accelleration and cognitive asynchrony, and, on the other hand, result from the adaptation to the changing conditions of how children function.
Monika Szczygieł, Krzysztof Cipora, Mateusz Hohol
Developmental Psychology, Volume 20, Issue 3, 2015, pp. 23-33
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.15.014.3803Finger counting plays an important role in mathematical cognition, especially in the acquisition of the concept of number and elementary math competence. Fingers are spontaneously used to count because of their constant availability and easiness of manipulation. Stable counting order within hand facilitates the acquisition of ordinal as well as cardinal numbers. Additionally, using fingers to count alleviates working memory load and allows constant control of counting accuracy. Apart from the usefulness for counting practice, cognitive representations of fingers are strongly interconnected with representations of numbers. Finger gnosis (the quality of the brain representations of fingers) is a good predictor of current as well as future math achievement. There is also evidence that the training of finger differentiation leads to improvements in math achievement.
Karolina Appelt
Developmental Psychology, Volume 20, Issue 3, 2015, pp. 37-51
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.15.015.3804The article presents the results of research on the quality of social environment for early childhood development and its subjective conditions. The research was conducted between 2010–2012. The participant group consisted of 972 children aged between 1 to 41 months and their parents of different socioeconomic status, inhabiting different parts of Poland. The research was carried out in their home space – the most natural environment for children. The IT-HOME/PL Inventory, a Polish adaptation of the American IT HOME Inventory (Infant/Todler Home Observation for Measurement of the Environment) by Betty M. Caldwell and Robert H. Bradley, was used. The results show that the social development environment differs visibly depending on the age of a child, which is coherent with the characteristics of the developmental phases presented in various concepts of human development. The first developmental period – infancy – finishes at the age of about one year, followed by early childhood in the second and third year of age. Each of these periods is characterized by different developmental needs of a small child requiring different organization of the developmental environment. However, according to the results achieved, the sex of the child is not a diversifying factor for the quality of social circles in which the child develops in the period of early childhood.
Paulina Michalska
Developmental Psychology, Volume 20, Issue 3, 2015, pp. 63-75
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.15.017.3805The study sought to investigate (1) the relationship between marital satisfaction and the value crisis among women, (2) the mediating role of the differentiation of Self. The sample consisted of 220 married women. The marital satisfaction was found to be inversely correlated with the value crisis. Greater marital satisfaction coincided with better differentiation of Self, while lower level of differentiation (less emotional reactivity, greater I position, less emotional cutoff and fusion with others) was significantly associated with higher difficulties in valuing. The differentiation of Self also mediated the linkage between the marital satisfaction and value crisis among women. The results support the searching hypothesis.
Kinga Kaleta
Developmental Psychology, Volume 20, Issue 3, 2015, pp. 63-75
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.15.017.3806The study sought to investigate (1) the relationship between marital satisfaction and the value crisis among women, (2) the mediating role of the differentiation of Self. The sample consisted of 220 married women. The marital satisfaction was found to be inversely correlated with the value crisis. Greater marital satisfaction coincided with better differentiation of Self, while lower level of differentiation (less emotional reactivity, greater I position, less emotional cutoff and fusion with others) was significantly associated with higher difficulties in valuing. The differentiation of Self also mediated the linkage between the marital satisfaction and value crisis among women. The results support the searching hypothesis.
Paweł Kurtek
Developmental Psychology, Volume 20, Issue 3, 2015, pp. 77-94
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.15.018.3807The paper is concerned on presentation the projective instrument (R-PDPI) to get acquainted with youth’s with mild intellectual disability coping with teacher’s or parental humiliation and dominance and also mates’ bullying and isolation. The cognitive concept of universal social stress was adopted to prepare the set of daily hassles (Tyszkowa, 1986; Hobfoll, 2006). The Content adequacy was checked by the competent judges (the Difference of Proportion Test for independent data). Then an empirical research in the target group of 17-21 years old made it possible to find the range of the coping strategies, declared by themselves, which were sorted comparatively to dimensions: active, directiveness and social costs (Hobfoll, 2006). Analyzes of discriminative value of type of dealing in each situations enable to find their scope and coherence. So the psychometric goodness of the instrument were proved.
Beata Winnicka
Developmental Psychology, Volume 20, Issue 3, 2015, pp. 97-102
Magdalena Kosno
Developmental Psychology, Volume 20, Issue 3, 2015, pp. 103-105
Publication date: 23.07.2015
Issue Editor: Dorota Kubicka
Wanda Zagórska, Anna Lipska
Developmental Psychology, Volume 20, Issue 2, 2015, pp. 11-25
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.15.007.3479The article concerns issues relating to family rituals, discussing them both from the theoretical findings, results of research and psychological functions, as well as therapeutic applications. After discussing the modern range of the concept of ritual, more than 60-year history of psychological research on family rituals in the world, mostly in the US, and the status of such research in Poland are presented. The existing definitions, classifications and attempts of operationalization of family rituals as well as research methods, including the structure of rituals and experiences gained from participation are discussed. In the final part the authors deal with psychological functions of family rituals.
Patryk Hajdo
Developmental Psychology, Volume 20, Issue 2, 2015, pp. 27-38
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.15.008.3480The process of transition from childhood to adulthood in primitive societies is connected with initiation rites, while in Western societies there are no such rituals. Instead, you can see conflicting expectations from adults directed towards the adolescents. They are ordering, depending on the circumstances, behavior consistent with the roles of a child or an adult. The content, contained in this form of communication, is the postulate of self-determination that satisfies the opposition. The adolescents’ response that satisfies these criteria is behaviour incompatible with adults’ expectations. The synchronic contradictory message, containing in its structure elements in accordance with the three phases of rites of passage, gives it the ability to read it in a diachronic accordance with the individual adolescent developmental process and thus corresponds to the initiation rites.
Katarzyna Skrzypińska, Paweł Atroszko
Developmental Psychology, Volume 20, Issue 2, 2015, pp. 41-56
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.15.009.3481Presented research concerns developmental aspect of human personality and spirituality. We compared study participants taking into account age and profession: 170 doctors, 48 managers, 81 monks and 561 persons from general population (N = 860; women 471). Two questionnaires were used: NEO-FFI (Costa, McCrae, 1992) to measure personality, and Self-Description Questionnaire (Kwestionariusz Samoopisu of Heszen-Niejodek,Gruszczyńska, 2004) to assess the essence and level of spirituality.
The results show that personality and spirituality (mainly religious attitude) allow to predict occupational pathways. Moreover age and dimensions of Big-Five Model (without openness) are predictors of spirituality, with age and Agreeableness being its best predictors. The results provide premises which indicate that personality, and spirituality as one of its dimensions, is an important factor related to choice of individual occupational pathway in life.
Barbara Gawda, Ewa Małgorzata Szepietowska
Developmental Psychology, Volume 20, Issue 2, 2015, pp. 57-72
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.15.010.3482The aim of this article is a description of content of emotional concepts. The main thesis was focused on quantitative and qualitative characteristics of affective concepts of Joy and Fear in 3 stages of adulthood (N = 302). The hypothesis was formulated about differences in numbers of generated words related to emotional concepts, and in structure of these concepts. Affective verbal fluency technique was used. Nouns were grouped in clusters for each concepts. Then, number of words in clusters and organization of clusters (cluster analysis) was compared between 3 stages of adulthood. There was shown some stable elements in each emotional concept, and some changeable aspects in concepts of Joy and Fear. Results are discussed in terms of development, and theories of emotional prototype or schemas.
Alan Mandal, Anna Latusek
Developmental Psychology, Volume 20, Issue 2, 2015, pp. 73-88
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.15.011.3483The aim of the research was the comparison the place attachment of people in various ages. Place attachment is a specific bond, positive emotional attitude, which is the result of compound units of this place (Altman, Low, 1992). We can distinguish two dimensions of attachment to the place: place identity and place dependence (Williams, Vaske, 2003). Relations between place attachment, age and life satisfaction of the respondents were examined.
The study involved 240 people, 4 groups over 60 people at the age of adolescence, early adulthood, middle adulthood and late adulthood. Place Attachment Scale (Williams, Vaske, 2003) and Satisfaction With Life Scale (Diener et al., 1985) were used.
Results showed that attachment to the place of residence positively correlated with age and satisfaction with life. Attachment to the place of residence is strongest in people in late adulthood, and weakest in early adulthood. For people in early adulthood and middle adulthood gender differences were reported: women were more strongly tied to the place than men.
Zdzisław Nieckarz, Dorota Godlewska-Werner, Sylwia Celińska-Nieckarz
Developmental Psychology, Volume 20, Issue 2, 2015, pp. 89-102
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.15.012.3484The aim of the study was to show that, despite the need for continuous professional development, the role of which is emphasized both in the individual (motivation to learn) and organizational (the need for efficiency), not all age groups in the area have the same experience. The study was conducted in a group of 167 employees of various institutions, using a questionnaire created for the study. The results show that age, as a determinant of developmental change may decide the choice of the type of educational activity, it also differentiates test takers in the range educational activity motives and barriers that are most relevant to them when choosing forms of learning. The process of directing their own development is therefore dependent not only on the preferences of the individual, which are an expression of subjectivity of human development but also the social context such as the support of the employer. Topic requires further studies that will take into account both the characteristics of the individual and the environment, without which it is impossible to fully define the developmental processes in the professional field.
Małgorzata Stępień-Nycz
Developmental Psychology, Volume 20, Issue 2, 2015, pp. 105-108
Dorota Katarzyna Suwalska-Barancewicz
Developmental Psychology, Volume 20, Issue 2, 2015, pp. 109-111
Publication date: 15.06.2015
Issue Editor: Joanna Kossewska
Jacek Kurpisz, Monika Mak, Katarzyna Nitsch, Ernest Tyburski
Developmental Psychology, Volume 20, Issue 1, 2015, pp. 11-22
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.15.001.3473Szymon Bręński
Developmental Psychology, Volume 20, Issue 1, 2015, pp. 23-37
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.15.005.3477Anna Oleszkowicz, Anna Misztela
Developmental Psychology, Volume 20, Issue 1, 2015, pp. 41-55
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.15.002.3474Beata Hintze
Developmental Psychology, Volume 20, Issue 1, 2015, pp. 57-74
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.15.003.3475Katarzyna Walęcka-Matyja, Dominika Kurpiel
Developmental Psychology, Volume 20, Issue 1, 2015, pp. 75-89
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.15.004.3476Dorota Katarzyna Suwalska-Barancewicz, Hanna Liberska, Paweł Kajetan Izdebski
Developmental Psychology, Volume 20, Issue 1, 2015, pp. 91-105
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.15.006.3478Justyna Konieczny, Magdalena Grzesik, Krzysztof Cipora
Developmental Psychology, Volume 20, Issue 1, 2015, pp. 109-113
Katarzyna Stawiarska
Developmental Psychology, Volume 20, Issue 1, 2015, pp. 115-121
Publication date: 11.03.2015
Editor of the Issue: Dorota Czyżowska
Janna M. Glozman
Developmental Psychology, Volume 19, Issue 4, 2014, pp. 9-14
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.14.021.2960Natalia Gawron, Katarzyna Jednoróg, Katarzyna Maria Bogdanowicz, Katarzyna Wiejak, Grażyna Krasowicz-Kupis
Developmental Psychology, Volume 19, Issue 4, 2014, pp. 15-30
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.14.022.2961Magdalena Jančina, Dorota Kubicka
Developmental Psychology, Volume 19, Issue 4, 2014, pp. 31-48
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.14.023.2962The aim of this article is to compare different conceptions of psychological development in which the development is managed by an individual itself. It is a collection of views on the development of the life of a human being from the point of view of different psychological trends. Terms such as self-realization, self-actualization and self-creation are considered. On the basis of the conception of self-creation by Zbigniew Pietrasiński, a model of selected but crucial personal conditions for self-creation process is discussed. It includes such dimensions as: autonomy as self-determination, self-consciousness, transgression, long-term goals and lifetime creativity.
Anna Izabela Brzezińska, Konrad Piotrowski, Tomasz Czub
Developmental Psychology, Volume 19, Issue 4, 2014, pp. 51-71
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.14.024.2963Małgorzata Rękosiewicz
Developmental Psychology, Volume 19, Issue 4, 2014, pp. 73-85
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.14.025.2964Renata Sikora, Elżbieta Greszta
Developmental Psychology, Volume 19, Issue 4, 2014, pp. 87-98
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.14.026.2965Maria Kielar-Turska
Developmental Psychology, Volume 19, Issue 4, 2014, pp. 101-111
Magdalena Kosno
Developmental Psychology, Volume 19, Issue 4, 2014, pp. 113-115
Publication date: 31.10.2014
Issue Editor: Dorota Kubicka
Paweł Socha
Developmental Psychology, Volume 19, Issue 3, 2014, pp. 9-22
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.14.015.2302Jarosław Groth, Marta Andrałojć
Developmental Psychology, Volume 19, Issue 3, 2014, pp. 23-36
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.14.016.2303Przemysław Bąbel, Paweł Ostaszewski, Monika Suchowierska
Developmental Psychology, Volume 19, Issue 3, 2014, pp. 37-47
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.14.017.2304Marta Białecka-Pikul, Małgorzata Karwala, Małgorzata Stępień-Nycz, Arkadiusz Białek
Developmental Psychology, Volume 19, Issue 3, 2014, pp. 51-68
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.14.018.2305The aim of the present analysis is to describe the developmental trajectory of the infant and toddler communicative competencies. Turn-taking, gaze-following, joint attention and pointing gestures are the most important expressions of these competencies that could be measured by the Early Social Communication Scale (ESCS Mundy et al., 2003). We present the results of a research conducted with 358 one-year old children and obtained through the Polish version of this scale (ESCS). The validity and reliability of the Polish version of ESCS proved to be satisfactory. The obtained results were analyzed emphasizing the role of early communicative competencies in the further social, linguistic and cognitive development of a child and indicating the domain of psychological practice where ESCS could be implemented.
Monika Dominiak-Kochanek, Adam Frączek
Developmental Psychology, Volume 19, Issue 3, 2014, pp. 69-84
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.14.019.2306The aim of the study was to define the role of corporal punishment experienced in childhood in the development of readiness for interpersonal aggression. 173 young adults assessed retrospectively if and how often they, as children, experienced the four following forms of corporal punishment: grabbing, spanking, pulling an ear or forcing to knee, and belting. Readiness for aggression was measured by The Readiness for Interpersonal Aggression Inventory, which enables to assess three distinct, intrapsychic mechanisms regulating aggressive acts: the emotional-impulsive readiness, the habitual-cognitive readiness and the personality-immanent readiness. The results show that corporal punishment used by fathers is a positive, significant predictor of all three patterns of readiness for interpersonal aggression in sons and daughters. No relation was found between corporal punishment by mothers and readiness for aggression of children, despite the fact that mothers used corporal punishment toward sons and daughters as frequent as fathers.
Elżbieta Talik, Kamila Król
Developmental Psychology, Volume 19, Issue 3, 2014, pp. 85-99
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.14.020.2307This study aims to test whether children with diverse frequency of school anxiety differ in coping strategies and whether the general self-efficacy moderates the relationship between school anxiety and coping strategies. 105 children aged 10–13 were examined with the Anxiety School Questionnaire by L. Czyżniejewska and G. Kopinke; the Self-Efficacy Scale by Z. Juczyński; and JSR-Questionnaire by Z. Juczyński and N. Ogińska-Bulik. The results show that children with higher frequency of school anxiety more often use emotional strategy to cope with stress. The moderating effect of the general self-efficacy on the relationship between school anxiety and coping strategies is not significant.
Natalia Józefacka
Developmental Psychology, Volume 19, Issue 3, 2014, pp. 103-108
Katarzyna Markiewicz
Developmental Psychology, Volume 19, Issue 3, 2014, pp. 109-114
Publication date: 25.09.2014
Issue Editor: Michał Grygielski
Andrzej Łukasik, Jan Rybak, Magdalena Marzec
Developmental Psychology, Volume 19, Issue 2, 2014, pp. 9-21
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.14.008.2286Barbara Harwas-Napierała
Developmental Psychology, Volume 19, Issue 2, 2014, pp. 23-32
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.14.009.2287Martyna Olbromska, Adam Putko
Developmental Psychology, Volume 19, Issue 2, 2014, pp. 33-48
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.14.010.2288Jan Rostowski, Teresa Rostowska
Developmental Psychology, Volume 19, Issue 2, 2014, pp. 49-65
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.14.011.2289Monika Marszał
Developmental Psychology, Volume 19, Issue 2, 2014, pp. 69-82
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.14.012.2290Aleksandra Peplińska, Piotr Połomski, Iwona Pogorzelska
Developmental Psychology, Volume 19, Issue 2, 2014, pp. 83-102
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.14.013.2291Monika Wysota, Aleksandra Pilarska, Katarzyna Adamczyk
Developmental Psychology, Volume 19, Issue 2, 2014, pp. 103-120
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.14.014.2292Iwona Sikorska
Developmental Psychology, Volume 19, Issue 2, 2014, pp. 123-126
Karolina Byczewska-Konieczny, Magdalena Kosno, Małgorzata Stępień-Nycz
Developmental Psychology, Volume 19, Issue 2, 2014, pp. 127-131
Publication date: 26.05.2014
Editor of the issue: Joanna Kossewska
Anna Rybka
Developmental Psychology, Volume 19, Issue 1, 2014, pp. 9-29
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.14.001.1723An increase in the epidemiological indicators and the neurodevelopmental character of autism cause more and more children to find their way into clinics dealing with early diagnosing and therapy. In view of the huge diversity of this population, the proper understanding of the nature of autism is crucial for a correct diagnosis. A survey of changes in the understanding of this disorder in the historical context may also broaden the perspective on the contemporary transformations of the diagnostic criteria. The paper presents modifications to the defining and classifying of autism over the years, from the description by Leo Kanner (1943) to the proposals included in DSM V (2013). Analysis of the changing diagnostic criteria shows that the first description of autism has, to a greater or lesser extent, had an impact on all authors of the subsequent criteria and classification textbooks. In some cases, the sets of the necessary and sufficient diagnostic criteria proposed within those 70 years of research on autism have had virtually nothing in common. On the one hand, it reflects the huge diversity of this group of people; on the other, it signifies that the question of what autism is has not found a definitive reply. Researchers are still discussing the nature of this disorder, its correct defi nition and diagnosis, as proven by the currently proposed changes in the diagnostic criteria.
Joanna Matuszczak-Świgoń
Developmental Psychology, Volume 19, Issue 1, 2014, pp. 33-50
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.14.002.1724The paper focuses on one aspect of the mother-child relation: the mother’s narration about her child during the prenatal period. The aim has been to establish how the mother builds a story about her developing child, which elements are common to most mothers’ narratives and which variables make the narratives different. In order to answer these questions, a narrative method has been used in a study involving 32 women between the 16th and 37th weeks of pregnancy.
Qualitative and quantitative analysis of the gathered stories shows that the mothers-to-be attribute a meaning and intentions not only to their children’s behaviours in their wombs but also to their own notions of those behaviours, and they base their reactions on them. Furthermore, the mothers arrange their expectations and beliefs concerning their children into coherent stories, which is a vital element of entering motherhood. In this way, they familiarize hemselves with the unknown and prepare themselves for performing the new role. In the annex to the paper, there are two narratives analysed thoroughly in terms of their content and structure.
Izabela Grzankowska, Małgorzata Anna Basińska
Developmental Psychology, Volume 19, Issue 1, 2014, pp. 51-66
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.14.003.1725One of the ways of presenting children’s experiences connected with their family environment is projection in drawing. The aim of this research has been to identify differences in children’s psychological profi les according to their position in the family birth order.
Children, including 179 girls (54%) and 151 boys (46%), have been surveyed using the Test of Projection Drawing, modifi ed by Maria Braun-Gałkowska, with the topic “Family”. Only 295 drawings have been analyzed, excluding drawings of middle children and twins.
The results show differences, conditioned by the family birth order and by the child’s sex, in the children’s psychological profi les in some aspects of personality.
Danuta Borecka-Biernat
Developmental Psychology, Volume 19, Issue 1, 2014, pp. 67-83
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.14.004.1726The aim of the research has been to determine the family aetiology of defensive coping strategies (aggression, avoidance, submission) employed by teenagers in social conflict situations and resulting from educational attitudes and model coping strategies chosen by their parents in analogous contexts. The following assessment methods have been used: the scale of parental attitudes by Mieczysław Plopa, the questionnaire of parental coping strategies in social conflict situations in the child’s perception (SRwSK) by Danuta Borecka-Biernat, and the questionnaire of adolescent coping strategies in social confl ict situations (KSRK) by D. Borecka-Biernat. The empirical study has covered 892 students (464 girls and 423 boys) aged 13–15, from the first, second and third years of the lower secondary school. The results show that adolescents are not able to deal constructively with the social confl ict situation unless in their educational environment they find approval, openness to their matters, recognition of their freedom of action, respect for their rights, and a model of active behaviour provided by signifi cant others while seeking a viable solution to a conflict. Defensive forms of coping strategies in social conflict situations develop under the influence of educational parental attitudes and models of reacting to problems, manifested in parents’ behaviours in emotional tension situations.
Beata Kostrubiec-Wojtachnio, Małgorzata Tatala
Developmental Psychology, Volume 19, Issue 1, 2014, pp. 85-102
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.14.005.1727The aim of the cognitive-developmental study has been to determine features of the image of God and indications of the relationship with God formed by adolescents. The definition of the image of God adopted in this paper has three aspects: (1) knowledge about God, (2) emotional experiences of God, and (3) behavioral tendencies towards God. It should be noted that the image of God is formed by imagination to a considerable degree, as we shape our mental representation of God on the basis of imagination. In the study, 114 adolescent Catholics were tested with the projection drawing “My God and I”. The position, shape, size, colors and the way of drawing the figure or symbol of God belong to the formal part of the analysis, while the meanings ascribed to the figure or symbol of God, i.e. the semantics of the representation, constitute the aspect of content. The results of the study have helped to isolate the dimensions of form and content in the image of God and to present their different categorizations.
Stanisław Głaz
Developmental Psychology, Volume 19, Issue 1, 2014, pp. 103-115
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.14.006.1728The aim of the research has been to present students’ preferences for values, their empathy and sense of security. The following methods have been used: the Rokeach Value Survey to identify the preference for values; Uchnast’s Sense of Security Questionnaire (SSQ) to determine the level of the sense of security, and Mehrabian and Epstein’s Empathy Questionnaire (MEEQ) to determine the level of empathy. The research was carried out in a group of students, aged 21–25, in Krakow in 2009. Further analysis was based on 200 sets of questionnaires filled in by the young people, with the average values (M), standard deviations (SD) and the ANOVA variance analysis taken into account and with the Multiple Stepwise Regression Analysis used to specify the independent variables. The research has shown that students with a high level of empathy prefer final values other than those chosen by students whose level of empathy is low. Students with both levels of empathy appreciate the personal final values of wisdom and pleasure more than social ones. The strongest final values helping to explain the variance of the sense of security in the group of students with a high level of empathy are personal values such as pleasure, comfortable life or exciting life, while in the group of students with a low level of empathy it is also a personal value, the beauty of the world.
Żaneta Stelter
Developmental Psychology, Volume 19, Issue 1, 2014, pp. 117-132
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.14.007.1729The paper presents the research aimed at determining the factors affecting the sense of marital satisfaction of parents of a child with intellectual disability. Special attention has been paid to the way in which the parental role is understood as well as to the parents’ description of their child. The research has involved 165 mothers and fathers of children with intellectual disability, aged 23–76. The following methods have been used: the Questionnaire of a Well-matched Couple (KDM-2), the Questionnaire My Child (KMD) and the Questionnaire of Fulfilling the Parental Role towards a Child with Intellectual Disability (KRRR). The results show that approval of the parental role and a positive way in which the parents describe their child belong to the factors supporting the parents’ marital satisfaction.
Katarzyna Stawiarska
Developmental Psychology, Volume 19, Issue 1, 2014, pp. 135-142
Monika Szczygieł, Krzysztof Cipora
Developmental Psychology, Volume 19, Issue 1, 2014, pp. 143-146
Publication date: 06.03.2014
Editor of the issue: Maria Kielar-Turska
Maria Kielar-Turska, Magdalena Kosno
Developmental Psychology, Volume 18, Issue 4, 2013, pp. 9-27
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.13.019.1720The paper presents an overview of basic issues related to executive functions, the phenomenon being the center of interest to psychologists at the turn of the 20th and 21st centuries, discussed from the historical, neurophysiological and ontogenetic perspectives. It gives basic information about the history of research on the executive functions and their neurophysiological correlates. At fi rst, the research focused on the function of the frontal cortex, which was thought to organize intellectual activity in adults with damage to that part of the brain. Studies in the ontogenetic paradigm have helped to explain the genesis of the executive functions, their complex nature, developmental changes and critical periods. They have also contributed to determining the relationship between the executive functions and other cognitive processes, such as theories of mind, or emotional processes, and to revealing the environmental conditions. The paper describes the complex nature of the executive functions, indicating the fixed components: inhibition, flexibility, working memory and planning, and presents psychological testing methods, pointing out the most commonly used techniques. The authors refer both to foreign and Polish authors studying groups of participants developing normally or with disorders in different periods of development, and they note the cognitive and the practical aspects of research on the executive functions.
Małgorzata Stępień-Nycz
Developmental Psychology, Volume 18, Issue 4, 2013, pp. 29-46
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.13.020.1721Recent interest in the executive functions has resulted in intensive research on the relation between these and other aspects of functioning, especially language and theory of mind. However, there are relatively few studies of the relation between the executive functions and emotional development, despite the fact that the executive functions are supposed to regulate not only thoughts and behaviour but also emotions (Best et al. 2009).
The paper presents a review of research on the relations between the executive functions and two aspects of the emotional development: emotion regulation and emotion understanding. The emotion regulation seems to be closely connected with the executive functions; in some contexts, these two constructs become undistinguishable, especially where the hot executive functions are concerned (Zelazo & Cunningham 2007). As we consider the cool aspect of the executive functions, research suggests that inhibition and cognitive flexibility may play a role in the emotion regulation. Similarly, inhibition and cognitive flexibility, together with working memory, seem to be related to the emotion understanding.
Finally, the paper quotes results suggesting a modifying influence of emotions on the executive functioning. Analysis of the mutual relations between the executive functioning and the emotional development has helped to elaborate the model of the relations between the control and the understanding in the domains of cognition and emotions (Leerkes et al. 2008).
Maria Kielar-Turska
Developmental Psychology, Volume 18, Issue 4, 2013, pp. 47-61
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.13.021.1722The paper discusses the relationship between language, cognition and behavior. It shows language as an external activator (the control function of speech) and an internal organizer of thoughts (private speech, inner speech). A. Luria’s research on the control function of speech has been continued in the study of the understanding of directives; L.S. Vygotsky’s concept of forms of speech has resulted in the imaginative research on private speech conducted since the 1980s, revealing the specifi city of private speech and the important part it plays in development, while studies of inner speech have helped to explain the role of language in thinking. Research on the executive functions allows us to track links between higher cognitive processes and language. It turns out that language and communication skills and vocabulary not only correlate with the developmental level of the executive functions, but are a good predictor of their development.
Adam Putko
Developmental Psychology, Volume 18, Issue 4, 2013, pp. 65-82
The main purpose of this study has been to verify a hypothesis based on the assumptions of the theories by Russell (1998) and Zelazo et al. (2005), concerning the relation between the development of executive functions (EF) and theory of mind (ToM). It was hypothesized that the level of the hot EF compared to the cool EF would be more strongly connected with children’s understanding of their own mental states than those of others. In the analysis of relationships between selected aspects of the EF and ToM, the level of language development was also considered. The study covered forty-four 3- and 4-year-olds, using false-belief tasks in their “unexpected contents” version (Gopnik & Astington, 1988) as the measures of ToM development, with one question requiring the attribution of a false belief to somebody else, and another question requiring the recognition of one’s own previous belief as false. The cool EF were measured with a version of the Bear-Dragon task (Reed et al., 1984), and the hot EF with the Children’s Gambling Task (Kerr, Zelazo, 2004). Language development was assessed with the Picture Vocabulary Test (OTS-R; Haman et al., 2011). It appeared that children’s understanding of their own mental states in both age groups was related only to the cool EF, while their understanding of other people’s mental states correlated marginally with the cool EF only in 4-year-olds. The level of the hot EF was not connected signifi cantly with any aspect of ToM. Regression analysis revealed that the cool aspect of the EF was a signifi cant language-independent predictor of the understanding of one’s own mental states in 4-year-olds, but not in 3-year-olds. The results are at variance with the predictions based on the theory by Zelazo et al., but consistent with those drawn from Russell’s theory, according to which self-consciousness and self-refl ection underlie the relationship between the EF and ToM.
Karolina Byczewska-Konieczny, Maria Kielar-Turska, Monika Paleczna
Developmental Psychology, Volume 18, Issue 4, 2013, pp. 83-93
Old age is characterized by an age-related cognitive decline. The role of executive functions in this process has recently been indicated. The population of elderly people, however, is not homogenous in terms of intellectual functioning. This diversity may be explained by evoking the notion of cognitive reserve which posits that elderly people have different levels of resources enabling them to compensate for the negative changes. The level of the cognitive reserve is related to the level of the executive functions.
This study analyzes the relationship between the levels of three components of the executive functions: flexibility, working memory and planning, and educational, cultural or physical activities carried out throughout life. The results show that educational and cultural activities are important to working memory, while planning is additionally helped by physical activity. Further, some components of the executive functions are more infl uenced by activity before retirement, while others are more affected by activity after retirement.
Magdalena Kosno
Developmental Psychology, Volume 18, Issue 4, 2013, pp. 95-110
The paper presents research on the relationship between different components and aspects of executive functions and the tutor’s behaviors and strategies used during peer tutoring. The research involved 23 pairs of six-year-old children. The tutors’ task was to teach their peers the rules of a board game. There was a signifi cant correlation between the level of the executive functions and the tutor’s behaviors and strategies which supported the student during the tutoring. Significant components of the executive functions included shifting attention, inhibition, working memory and the cool aspect of the executive functions. The tutors applying various strategies (Outside the role, Initiator, Partner, Coordinator) differed signifi cantly in their levels of the executive functions, and especially in the planning component.
Maria Kielar-Turska
Developmental Psychology, Volume 18, Issue 4, 2013, pp. 113-122
Sprawozdanie z International Workshop on Bilingulism and Cognitive Control, Uniwersytet Jagielloński
Justyna Kotowicz
Developmental Psychology, Volume 18, Issue 4, 2013, pp. 123-130
Publication date: 16.01.2014
Editor of the issue: Dorota Czyżowska
Przemysław Bąbel, Elżbieta A. Bajcar, Anna M. Ziółkowska
Developmental Psychology, Volume 18, Issue 3, 2013, pp. 9-19
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.13.013.1467In 1920 John B. Watson and Rosalie Rayner published the results of the study experiment describing how they had conditioned an 11-month-old boy (known as Little Albert) to fear a rat. The experiment is one of the best known and the most frequently cited empirical studies in the history of psychology. Many studies and theories suggesting the role of learning processes in the development of emotional responses were initiated by the Little Albert experiment. The article summarizes the procedures and results of the experiment reported by J.B. Watson and R. Rayner. The importance and impact of the results of the experiment on the development of psychological theories and research is discussed. Errors in the discussions of the Little Albert experiment in Polish psychological literature are identifi ed. The results of the latest historical research on the Little Albert experiment are summarized and their consequences are discussed.
Joanna Kossewska, Brenda Peters, Petra Potměŝilová, Miloň Potměšil
Developmental Psychology, Volume 18, Issue 3, 2013, pp. 23-36
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.13.014.1468Hearing impairment is regarded to be a barrier to the psychological development and language skills of deaf students. In understanding and establishing what comprises the value priorities amongst adolescents, certain deviations can be expected amongst teenagers who are deaf or who have hearing impairment. The article describes and compares the value priorities of 246 adolescents who are deaf or with hearing impairment from Poland and the Czech Republic with hearing peers. The Schwartz Portrait Values Questionnaire is used to measure the value priorities of the respective groups of participants. In comparison to peers responses the results indicate that hearing status impacts upon an individuals’ values of conformity, tradition as well as achievement, however, other value priorities are shown to be infl uenced by gender and country of domicile.
Aneta R. Borkowska, Piotr Francuz
Developmental Psychology, Volume 18, Issue 3, 2013, pp. 37-50
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.13.015.1469In this paper we discuss the problem whether the disorder of the development of spelling awareness visible in the form of spelling errors occurs at the level of the visual perception of words. It was assumed that, in an eye-tracker study, adolescents aged 13–15 years with a spelling disorder (N = 32) will have a different pattern of eye movement in the task of evaluating the correctness of spelling in word and pseudo-word tasks than good spellers (N = 19).
The main result was the lack of inter-group differences with regard to most indicators of eye movement. This means that both groups examine the words with a spelling diffi culty in a similar, visual way. There is no difference between the level of engagement and direction of visual attention. The disorder of the development of spelling awareness, therefore, is not the result of the problems with perception and attention.
Ryszard Poprawa
Developmental Psychology, Volume 18, Issue 3, 2013, pp. 51-73
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.13.016.1470Previous research based on Jan Strelaus (2006) Regulative Theory of Temperament (RTT) shows that energetic traits of temperament and non-harmonization of its structure are of particular signifi cance to the risk of development of problems with alcohol drinking in late adolescents and young adults. The purpose of this study was to verify the results of previous research. The degree of alcohol use involvement constituted the variable explained, whereas temperament traits and their structure constituted the predictor variables. The degree of alcohol use involvement was measured with the use of Ryszard Poprawa’s (2013) Alcohol Use Scale (AUS). Temperament traits according to RTT were measured with the use of Bogdan Zawadzki and Jan Strelau’s (1997) Formal Characteristics of Behaviour – Temperament Inventory (FCB-TI). A sample of 208 adolescents and young adults at the age of 16 to 20 years old were interviewed. As a result of a multiple regression analysis, two sex-dependent models of the AUS temperamental predictor were selected. The temperamental variables determine the AUS results to a greater degree when it comes to young men rather than women. Together with the lower sensory sensitivity, emotional reactivity and endurance, and higher activity in men comes their deeper alcohol use involvement. Together with the higher activity and emotional reactivity, and lower sensory sensitivity in women comes their deeper alcohol use involvement. Based on the cluster analysis, there were selected groups of young men and women whose structure of temperament traits on the one hand predispose them to deep alcohol use involvement and on the other hand might protect them against risky involvement . In contrast to previous research, it was proved that low sensory sensitivity is of crucial importance to the degree of alcohol use involvement. It has not been fully proved that the lack of temperament structure harmonization has any particular significance to the risk of addiction in young women.
Aleksandra Tłuściak-Deliowska
Developmental Psychology, Volume 18, Issue 3, 2013, pp. 75-86
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.13.017.1471Studies on bullying have typically concentrated only on the bully-victim relationship. It is as if bullying behavior is regarded only as a function of certain characteristics of the bully and/or the victim, while the group context is set aside, or forgotten. Meanwhile bullying is a group process, a social phenomenon, and each group member could assume a different role. The aim of the study was to examine different participant roles taken by individual adolescents in the bullying process, as well as the relation between the sex of the adolescent and the declared behavior. The study was conducted on a sample of 214 students. The participant roles assigned to the subjects were: reinforcer and assistant of the bully, passive bystander, outsider and defender of the victim. There were significant sex differences in the distribution of participant roles.
Dorota Kubicka
Developmental Psychology, Volume 18, Issue 3, 2013, pp. 87-108
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.13.018.1472The paper presents the results of research on identification of creative preschoolers via observation of their overt behavior in two settings: at home and in the kindergarten. A model of preschoolers’ creative activity as well as two original observational sheets were prepared on the basis of the knowledge of the subject. The data were collected form a sample of parents and teachers of 235 preschoolers.
The main categories of creative activity at home were: humour, autonomy, self-expression, persistence, fantasy/imagination, stubbornness; in the kindergarten they were: exploration, control/autonomy, engagement. The results show that the relationship between the parents’ and teachers’ ratings was very weak. The children exhibited a wide range of creative behaviors across the main categories rather than a full pattern (syndrome) of creative behavior. These findings support the existence of a large and at the same time dispersed creative potential of preschoolers, very diversified patterns of creative behaviors, and small developmental changes within this range.
Karolina Byczewska-Konieczny
Developmental Psychology, Volume 18, Issue 3, 2013, pp. 111-114
Karolina Byczewska-Konieczny, Magdalena Kosno, Małgorzata Stępień-Nycz
Developmental Psychology, Volume 18, Issue 3, 2013, pp. 115-119
Publication date: 29.06.2013
Editor of the issue: Dorota Kubicka
Piotr Olesiński
Developmental Psychology, Volumwe 18, Issue 2, 2013, pp. 9-18
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.13.007.1180Lucyna Bakiera
Developmental Psychology, Volumwe 18, Issue 2, 2013, pp. 21-33
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.13.008.1181Agnieszka Zając
Developmental Psychology, Volumwe 18, Issue 2, 2013, pp. 35-47
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.13.009.1182Sybilla Schiep, Katarzyna Cieślik, Karolina Fila, Barbara Bętkowska-Korpała
Developmental Psychology, Volumwe 18, Issue 2, 2013, pp. 49-64
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.13.010.1183Marta Łockiewicz, Karol Karasiewicz, Marta Bogdanowicz, Katarzyna Maria Bogdanowicz
Developmental Psychology, Volumwe 18, Issue 2, 2013, pp. 65-78
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.13.011.1184Alicja Malina, Dorota Katarzyna Suwalska-Barancewicz
Developmental Psychology, Volumwe 18, Issue 2, 2013, pp. 79-90
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.13.012.1185Katarzyna Stawiarska
Developmental Psychology, Volumwe 18, Issue 2, 2013, pp. 93-100
Magdalena Kosno
Developmental Psychology, Volumwe 18, Issue 2, 2013, pp. 101-106
Publication date: 29.06.2013
Editor of the issue: Michał Grygielski
Miloň Potměšil, Jiří Pospíšil
Developmental Psychology, Volume 18, Issue 1, 2013, pp. 9-25
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.13.001.1013Tomasz Czub, Anna Izabela Brzezińska
Developmental Psychology, Volume 18, Issue 1, 2013, pp. 27-42
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.13.002.1014Elżbieta Napora
Developmental Psychology, Volume 18, Issue 1, 2013, pp. 45-56
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.13.003.1015Nina Ogińska-Bulik, Magdalena Zadworna-Cieślak
Developmental Psychology, Volume 18, Issue 1, 2013, pp. 57-68
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.13.004.1016Anna Siudem
Developmental Psychology, Volume 18, Issue 1, 2013, pp. 69-85
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.13.005.1017
Risky behaviours of youth at secondary-school age
In the face of different civilization changes threatening human health and life, risky behaviours of youth have become one of the essential research areas of present-day psychology. The conclusions of this research can be used to design programmes and preventive measures in this area. The purpose of this research was to determine the structure of risky behaviours of youth at secondary-school age. It was assumed that within a group of youth from the second class of the secondary school, most of the risky behaviours associated with the use of alcohol, nicotine and drugs occurred at least once during the 12 months preceding the research. It was also assumed that sex differentiates risky behaviours in terms of their frequency and structure. The research was conducted on a group of youth of the second class of secondary school. This group consisted of 158 people. To determine the risky behaviours the diagnostic survey method was used. The conclusions of the research made it possible to outline a picture of the spread of these dysfunctions in the school environment of the youth. The vast majority of the respondents declared that during the 12 months preceding the survey, they most frequently used alcohol. Only one out of ten respondents claimed to have abstained from alcohol. Also popular was the use of tobacco and cannabis (marijuana and hashish). The most signifi cant differences between girls and boys occurred primarily in the field of tobacco use, with the frequency of ‘1–5 cigarettes daily’ in favor of girls.
Dominika Karaś, Maria Kłym, Jan Cieciuch
Developmental Psychology, Volume 18, Issue 1, 2013, pp. 87-101
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.13.006.1018Maria Kielar-Turska
Developmental Psychology, Volume 18, Issue 1, 2013, pp. 105-113
Justyna Kotowicz
Developmental Psychology, Volume 18, Issue 1, 2013, pp. 115-118
Publication date: 07.01.2013
Editor of the issue: Joanna Kossewska
Barbara M. Kaja
Developmental Psychology, Volume 17, Issue 4, 2012, pp. 9-22
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.12.022.0787Agnieszka Lasota
Developmental Psychology, Volume 17, Issue 4, 2012, pp. 25-35
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.12.023.0788Joanna Lessing-Pernak
Developmental Psychology, Volume 17, Issue 4, 2012, pp. 37-44
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.12.024.0789Anna Kołodziejczyk
Developmental Psychology, Volume 17, Issue 4, 2012, pp. 45-62
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.12.025.0790Paweł Kurtek
Developmental Psychology, Volume 17, Issue 4, 2012, pp. 63-77
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.12.026.0791Agnieszka Szymańska
Developmental Psychology, Volume 17, Issue 4, 2012, pp. 79-91
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.12.027.0792Anna Rybka
Developmental Psychology, Volume 17, Issue 4, 2012, pp. 95-103
Joanna Kossewska
Developmental Psychology, Volume 17, Issue 4, 2012, pp. 105-110
Publication date: 19.11.2012
Editor of the issue: Dorota Kubicka
Maria Mądry
Developmental Psychology, Volume 17, Issue 3, 2012, pp. 9-21
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.12.015.0634Zuzanna Siwek
Developmental Psychology, Volume 17, Issue 3, 2012, pp. 25-38
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.12.016.0635Kinga Kaleta
Developmental Psychology, Volume 17, Issue 3, 2012, pp. 39-55
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.12.017.0636Dorota Czyżowska, Ewa Gurba, Arkadiusz Białek
Developmental Psychology, Volume 17, Issue 3, 2012, pp. 57-68
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.12.018.0637Marta Maćkiewicz
Developmental Psychology, Volume 17, Issue 3, 2012, pp. 69-82
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.12.019.0638Katarzyna Barani, Barbara Szmigielska
Developmental Psychology, Volume 17, Issue 3, 2012, pp. 83-95
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.12.020.0639Sybilla Schiep, Agnieszka Szymańska
Developmental Psychology, Volume 17, Issue 3, 2012, pp. 97-113
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.12.021.0640Marzanna Farnicka
Developmental Psychology, Volume 17, Issue 3, 2012, pp. 117-120
Agnieszka Lasota
Developmental Psychology, Volume 17, Issue 3, 2012, pp. 121-125
Publication date: 11.06.2012
Editor of the issue: Dorota Czyżowska
Jowita Wycisk
Developmental Psychology, Volume 17, Issue 2, 2012, pp. 9-19
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.12.009.0458Maria Kaźmierczak, Bogumiła Kiełbratowska, Aleksandra Lewandowska-Walter, Justyna Michałek, Magdalena Błażek
Developmental Psychology, Volume 17, Issue 2, 2012, pp. 23-39
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.12.010.0459Renata Sikora
Developmental Psychology, Volume 17, Issue 2, 2012, pp. 41-54
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.12.011.0460Dorota Czyżowska, Kamila Mikołajewska
Developmental Psychology, Volume 17, Issue 2, 2012, pp. 55-70
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.12.012.0461Alicja Malina, Dorota Suwalska
Developmental Psychology, Volume 17, Issue 2, 2012, pp. 71-86
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.12.013.0462Katarzyna Adamczyk
Developmental Psychology, Volume 17, Issue 2, 2012, pp. 87-100
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.12.014.0463Ewa Gurba, Małgorzata Stępień-Nycz
Developmental Psychology, Volume 17, Issue 2, 2012, pp. 103-107
Publication date: 23.05.2012
Editor of the issue: Maria Kielar-Turska
Janusz Trempała
Developmental Psychology, Volume 17, Issue 1, 2012, pp. 17-29
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.12.001.0376Maria Flis
Developmental Psychology, Volume 17, Issue 1, 2012, pp. 31-38
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.12.002.0377The primary thesis of this article is in accord with the postulate that notions about mankind are rooted in the concept of human nature. From the perspective of the history of ideas, it is clearly evident that the essence of the human being is intertwined with history while the influence of this intrinsic understanding of man has been and continues to exceed that of history. The strongest evidence of this is found in the philosophy of the human individual constantly present in the psychological discourse despite the intervening, temporary dominance of the empirical paradigm. The belief that everything can be measured and counted stems from mythical thought. This text undertakes an attempt to categorize the conceptual apparatus of contemporary cognitive science. Concurrently, it indicates the definitions and relationships which encompass such concepts as the mind, consciousness, and thinking.
Marek Drwięga
Developmental Psychology, Volume 17, Issue 1, 2012, pp. 39-47
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.12.003.0378Andrzej Łukasik
Developmental Psychology, Volume 17, Issue 1, 2012, pp. 49-64
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.12.004.0379Grażyna Mendecka
Developmental Psychology, Volume 17, Issue 1, 2012, pp. 65-76
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.12.005.0380Ewa Nowińska
Developmental Psychology, Volume 17, Issue 1, 2012, pp. 77-84
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.12.006.0381Krzysztof Jodzio
Developmental Psychology, Volume 17, Issue 1, 2012, pp. 85-98
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.12.007.0382Tomasz Frąckowiak
Developmental Psychology, Volume 17, Issue 1, 2012, pp. 101-115
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.12.008.0383Magdalena Kosno
Developmental Psychology, Volume 17, Issue 1, 2012, pp. 117-121
Kamil Jezierski
Developmental Psychology, Volume 17, Issue 1, 2012, pp. 126-126
Publication date: 14.02.2012
Issue editor: Michał Grygielski
Julius Kuhl
Developmental Psychology, Volume 16, Issue 4, 2011, pp. 9-31
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.11.021.0194The developmental precursors of the “fully functioning person” (Rogers) have not received much systematic attention neither in psychological research nor in theories of personality and its development. Likewise, the developmental conditions for maladaptive personality functioning and the formation of psychological symptoms have not been the primary target of research in clinical psychology during the past decades (Castonguay, 2011). On the basis of a theory of personality that specifies a functional architecture of personality (i.e., personality systems interactions theory: PSI theory) developmental pathways are described that promote successful or impaired development of the integrated self which plays a pivotal role in PSI theory. Adaptive developmental pathways are described in terms of the developmental conditions promoting the ability to initiate unattractive instrumental activities (action control) and the integration of self-alien and painful experiences into an ever growing personal knowledge base (self-development). Maladaptive pathways are proposed to describe the developmental conditions for two major forms of psychological disorders: symptoms related to impaired action control (e.g., procrastination, eating disorders, depression) and symptoms related to impaired self-growth (e.g., rigidity, failure to learn from mistakes, psychosomatic symptoms). Any theory of personality should be able to explain adaptive and maladaptive forms of personality development. However, this issue does not belong to the prime targets of research efforts made in personality, developmental, or clinical psychology. Instead, competing hypotheses are derived from global constructs such as self-effi cacy, irrational thinking, poor self-esteem and the like. Typical questions arising from this globalconstruct approach are: Does a man who has problems initiating simple activities at home (despite his effi cient performance at his work) suffer from impaired self-efficacy, poor selfesteem, or does he simply have chauvinistic attitudes against his wife? Global constructs are also offered by the two major schools of thinking in psychology: From a psychoanalytic point of view, a woman’s overeating may be caused by an “oral regression” which dictates her behavior to eat as impulsively as oral needs drive baby’s feelings during the oral stage of development? In contrast, behavioristic learning theory would attribute those and other symptoms such as disturbed sleep, impaired immune function and stress-dependent pain like headache or backpain to the acquisition of irrational thinking or maladaptive behaviors that can be remedied by rewarding more appropriate behaviors.
Malwina Szpitalak
Developmental Psychology, Volume 16, Issue 4, 2011, pp. 47-53
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.11.022.0195Anna Słysz
Developmental Psychology, Volume 16, Issue 4, 2011, pp. 47-53
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.11.023.0196Małgorzata Stępień-Nycz
Developmental Psychology, Volume 16, Issue 4, 2011, pp. 57-72
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.11.024.0197Dorota Kubicka
Developmental Psychology, Volume 16, Issue 4, 2011, pp. 73-87
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.11.025.0198The article discusses the topic of children’s creativity and its research presented in the book Creative Action in Preschool Children (Kubicka, 2003). The book describes the projective method of measuring creative strategies in symbolic-constructive play. In a 1997 sample, 120 children aged 3 to 6 were tested. 12 years later a second version of the method was worked out, and a group of 18 of both the most and the least creative of the original sample was tested again. The pupils were also interviewed for all facts and questions important for the development of creativity both at preschool age and in adolescence. The results show many determinants of the prognostic value of the proposed method and warn against making conclusions about child’s creativity on the basis of simple paper and pencil tests. At the same time the fi ndings suggest the need for more dynamic complex developmental approach to diagnosis of creativity in children in order to uncover the mechanisms of self-development of these functions.
Romana Kadzikowska-Wrzosek
Developmental Psychology, Volume 16, Issue 4, 2011, pp. 89-108
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.11.026.0199Karolina Byczewska-Konieczny, Magdalena Kosno, Małgorzata Stępień-Nycz
Developmental Psychology, Volume 16, Issue 4, 2011, pp. 111-119
Karolina Byczewska-Konieczny
Developmental Psychology, Volume 16, Issue 4, 2011, pp. 121-125
Publication date: 09.10.2011
Issue editors: Marta Białecka-Pikul, Przemysław Bąbel
Marta Białecka-Pikul
Developmental Psychology, Volume 16, Issue 3, 2011, pp. 15-25
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.11.015.0188Przemysław Bąbel
Developmental Psychology, Volume 16, Issue 3, 2011, pp. 27-38
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.11.016.0189Monika Zielińska
Developmental Psychology, Volume 16, Issue 3, 2011, pp. 39-49
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.11.017.0190Rafał Kawa
Developmental Psychology, Volume 16, Issue 3, 2011, pp. 51-60
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.11.018.0191Monika Zielińska, Aneta Bagińska
Developmental Psychology, Volume 16, Issue 3, 2011, pp. 63-74
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.11.019.0192Ewa Pisula, Dorota Noińska
Developmental Psychology, Volume 16, Issue 3, 2011, pp. 75-88
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.11.020.0193Anna M. Ziółkowska
Developmental Psychology, Volume 16, Issue 3, 2011, pp. 91-95
Marta Rynda
Developmental Psychology, Volume 16, Issue 3, 2011, pp. 97-101
Publication date: 19.09.2011
Issue editors: Dorota Czyżowska, Dorota Turska
Dorota Turska
Developmental Psychology, Volume 16, Issue 2, 2011, pp. 13-20
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.11.008.0181The educational aims of the educational reform which is now well underway come in the form of measurable and material indicators such as the results of the external test, educational added value or test pass rate with respect to the secondary school final examinations. There is no doubt that until recently such terminology was reserved for economics. It is worth remembering that the primary aim of education, as declared in the Education Act, is “to provide an impulse to full development of each student”. By its nature, such an aim is non-measurable and intangible. The paper shows that education subordinated to external examinations is a barrier to the comprehensive development of the young as defined in Jung’s theory. Basic functions concerning reception and valuation of information in relation to the formal requirements laid down for the secondary school final examination in Polish were analysed. The nature of these requirements may lead to the dominance of sensing-thinking type in the comprehensive cognition of a human personality. Other preferences may prove useless or simply harmful. Jung’s theory helps point out that educational requirements subordinated to measurable indicators are painfully one-sided. Consequently, they pose a serious risk of developmental fragmentation.
Sławomir Trusz
Developmental Psychology, Volume 16, Issue 2, 2011, pp. 21-32
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.11.009.0182Aleksandra Jasielska, Renata Maksymiuk
Developmental Psychology, Volume 16, Issue 2, 2011, pp. 33-49
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.11.010.0183For market specialists no group of consumers is lost. That is why the developmental task of taking on a parental role is automatically combined with the commercialization of parenting. This phenomenon relies on the redefinition of motherhood and fatherhood in terms of the market. Two main tendencies can be observed. The first one is connected with the important description of a parent as a consumer, e.g. treating them as investors, or relating to a typology of shopping mothers. The second one is connected with using psychology for projecting consumer behaviors, e.g. the analysis of parents’ vulnerability to the nag factor or milestone marketing. The possible consequences and the ways to control the commercialization of parenting will be discussed.
Dorota Czyżowska, Agnieszka Leśniak
Developmental Psychology, Volume 16, Issue 2, 2011, pp. 53-66
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.11.011.0184The main aim of the present investigation is to gain knowledge about teachers’ beliefs concerning the possibility of modeling pupils’ moral character. The main components in the process of moral education are a teacher’s personality, awareness of their role in education, their skills and competencies and belief in the possibility of infl uencing their pupils. In view of these facts it has been agreed that there should be conducted a study to examine how teachers assess their own abilities and skills in modeling their pupils’ character, and how they see teachers’ overall possibilities in this area.
The research used a questionnaire “Character Education Effi cacy Belief Instrument” developed by Milson and Mehlig (2002). This tool measures teachers’ beliefs concerning their effectiveness of modeling pupils’ moral character. There are two scales used in this measurement. The first scale allows to assess teachers’ personal effectiveness of teaching (personal teaching efficacy – PTE) and the second one allows to assess general effectiveness of teaching (general teaching efficacy – GTE). There were 218 people involved in the research (83% were women, and 17% men). The results show a signifi cant difference between the assessment of personal teaching effectiveness (PTE) and general teaching effectiveness (GTE). Respondents (teachers) evaluate their own abilities in modeling their pupils’ moral characters higher than the general ability of teachers in this respect. Three factors were taken into account in the analysis: teacher’s gender, type of school where the respondents work and subjects they teach.
Olga Bąk
Developmental Psychology, Volume 16, Issue 2, 2011, pp. 67-82
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.11.012.0185Michał Kierzkowski
Developmental Psychology, Volume 16, Issue 2, 2011, pp. 83-96
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.11.013.0186Dorota Turska
Developmental Psychology, Volume 16, Issue 2, 2011, pp. 99-102
Grażyna Katra
Developmental Psychology, Volume 16, Issue 2, 2011, pp. 103-106
Publication date: 04.09.2011
Editor of the issue: Maria Kielar-Turska
Stająca się dorosłość w ujęciu Jeffreya J. Arnetta jako rozbudowana faza liminalna rytuału przejścia
Anna Lipska, Wanda Zagórska
Developmental Psychology, Volume 16, Issue 1, 2011, pp. 9-21
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.11.001.0174Hanna Liberska, Dorota Suwalska
Developmental Psychology, Volume 16, Issue 1, 2011, pp. 25-39
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.11.002.0175Attachment is the basis for the creation of close interpersonal relationships. The destination of the analysis was to identify whether there is a link between attachment style and partner relationships and to determine whether factors such as: a form of relationship, gender, length of relationship, education, having children, number of previous relationships are important to the quality of relationships and the attachment partners’s style.
The study involved 120 people (marriage, engagement, cohabitation). Used “The Intimacy, Passion, Commitment Questionnaire” by Acker and Davis (1992, Wojciszke, 2005) expanded by an additional scale of “satisfaction from the relationship,” Partner Relations Questionnaire” by Kurt Hahlweg and “The Attatchment Style Questionnaire” for Mieczysław Plopa.
The results showed that the attachment style of partners is important for their relationship. It was found that the form and length of relationship, number of previous partners, children, education and gender are important for some relationship partners. The form of relationship and number of previous partners are also important for the secure attachment style. The obtained data showed that the attachment styles affect the quality of relationships.
Alicja Malina
Developmental Psychology, Volume 16, Issue 1, 2011, pp. 41-55
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.11.003.0176Katarzyna Palus
Developmental Psychology, Volume 16, Issue 1, 2011, pp. 57-71
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.11.004.0177Adam Putko
Developmental Psychology, Volume 16, Issue 1, 2011, pp. 73-84
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.11.005.0178Karolina Byczewska-Konieczny, Maria Kielar-Turska
Developmental Psychology, Volume 16, Issue 1, 2011, pp. 85-98
https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.11.006.0179Dorota Turska
Developmental Psychology, Volume 16, Issue 1, 2011, pp. 99-104
Justyna Kotowicz
Developmental Psychology, Volume 16, Issue 1, 2011, pp. 105-108
Publication date: 29.01.2011
Issue editor: Dorota Czyżowska
Michael D. Berzonsky
Developmental Psychology, Volume 15, Issue 4, 2010, pp. 13-27
Alicja Senejko
Developmental Psychology, Volume 15, Issue 4, 2010, pp. 31-48
Jan Cieciuch
Developmental Psychology, Volume 15, Issue 4, 2010, pp. 49-64
Anna Oleszkowicz, Anna Misztela
Developmental Psychology, Volume 15, Issue 4, 2010, pp. 65-77
Ewa Gurba
Developmental Psychology, Volume 15, Issue 4, 2010, pp. 79-92
Kamil Jezierski
Developmental Psychology, Volume 15, Issue 4, 2010, pp. 93-102
Michał Andrzej Golombek
Developmental Psychology, Volume 15, Issue 4, 2010, pp. 105-107
Tomasz Frąckowiak
Developmental Psychology, Volume 15, Issue 4, 2010, pp. 109-112
Publication date: 14.12.2010
Issue editor: Maria Kielar-Turska
Maria Kielar-Turska
Developmental Psychology, Volume 15, Issue 3, 2010, pp. 7-8
Developmental Psychology, Volume 15, Issue 3, 2010, pp. 11-17
Marta Kochan-Wójcik, Joanna Piskorz
Developmental Psychology, Volume 15, Issue 3, 2010, pp. 21-32
Nina Ogińska-Bulik
Developmental Psychology, Volume 15, Issue 3, 2010, pp. 33-43
Kinga Kaleta
Developmental Psychology, Volume 15, Issue 3, 2010, pp. 43-56
Katarzyna Markiewicz, Bożydar L. J. Kaczmarek, Małgorzata Kostka-Szymańska
Developmental Psychology, Volume 15, Issue 3, 2010, pp. 57-70
Małgorzata Niesiobędzka
Developmental Psychology, Volume 15, Issue 3, 2010, pp. 71-80
Anna Szczypczyk
Developmental Psychology, Volume 15, Issue 3, 2010, pp. 83-88
Małgorzata Stępień-Nycz
Developmental Psychology, Volume 15, Issue 3, 2010, pp. 89-94
Małgorzata Kaczor
Developmental Psychology, Volume 15, Issue 3, 2010, pp. 95-97
Publication date: 29.11.2010
Issue editor: Michał Grygielski
Luciano L‛Abate
Developmental Psychology, Volume 15, Issue 2, 2010, pp. 9-17
Malwina Szpitalak
Developmental Psychology, Volume 15, Issue 2, 2010, pp. 19-30
Anna K. Döring, Jan Cieciuch , Justyna Harasimczuk
Developmental Psychology, Volume 15, Issue 2, 2010, pp. 33-45
Dorota Czyżowska, Anna Oleszkowicz
Developmental Psychology, Volume 15, Issue 2, 2010, pp. 47-59
Zbigniew Łoś
Developmental Psychology, Volume 15, Issue 2, 2010, pp. 61-75
Monika Kozłowska
Developmental Psychology, Volume 15, Issue 2, 2010, pp. 77-88
Dominika Świerżewska
Developmental Psychology, Volume 15, Issue 2, 2010, pp. 89-99
Iwona Janicka
Developmental Psychology, Volume 15, Issue 2, 2010, pp. 103-109
Dagmara Musiał
Developmental Psychology, Volume 15, Issue 2, 2010, pp. 111-113
Publication date: 31.12.2009
Issue editor: Maria Kielar-Turska
Edward Łuczyński
Developmental Psychology, Volume 15, Issue 1, 2010, pp. 9-18
Ewa Haman, Krzysztof Fronczyk, Aneta Miękisz
Developmental Psychology, Volume 15, Issue 1, 2010, pp. 21-45
Maria Kielar-Turska, Agnieszka Lasota
Developmental Psychology, Volume 15, Issue 1, 2010, pp. 47-60
Magdalena Kosno
Developmental Psychology, Volume 15, Issue 1, 2010, pp. 61-73
Anna Kołodziejczyk
Developmental Psychology, Volume 15, Issue 1, 2010, pp. 75-88
Magdalena Piełuć, Małgorzata Fludra
Developmental Psychology, Volume 15, Issue 1, 2010, pp. 89-97
Magdalena Kosno
Developmental Psychology, Volume 15, Issue 1, 2010, pp. 101-103
Karolina Małek
Developmental Psychology, Volume 15, Issue 1, 2010, pp. 105-110
Publication date: 2009
Issue Editor: Dorota Czyżowska
Michael D. Berzonsky
Developmental Psychology, Volume 14, Issue 4, 2009, pp. 9-18
Barbara Szmigielska, Marta Tomaszek
Developmental Psychology, Volume 14, Issue 4, 2009, pp. 21-31
Aneta Chybicka, Karol Karasiewicz
Developmental Psychology, Volume 14, Issue 4, 2009, pp. 33-43
The aim of the research was to determine whether relational satisfaction is dependent on the type of relation (engagement, marriage relations of different duration: up to three years and over ten years), the quality of communication between partners, and the level of the endorsement of partners’ opinions concerning the stereotypical woman’s role in a close relationship. Two hundred sixteen engaged and married couples took part in the research. Seventy two couples were engaged, seventy two couples were married and the duration of the marriage did not exceed 3 years, and seventy two couples were married for a period longer than 10 years. Participants completed The Questionnaire of Marriage Communication (Kaźmierczak, Plopa, 2005), the level of endorsement of the partners’ opinions concerning stereotypical woman’s role in a close relationship and relational satisfaction were also measured. The results show higher relational satisfaction of participants engaged. Moreover, it was observed that the longer the relation, the lower the assessment of relational satisfaction, and the worse the quality of communication. The important factors determining relational satisfaction are both the quality of communication and the level of endorsement of partners’ opinions concerning stereotypical feminine role in a relationship. Obtained results were analysed in the light of modern theories of relations and gender roles
Magdalena Grabowska
Developmental Psychology, Volume 14, Issue 4, 2009, pp. 45-56
The article aims to present results of the research on the infl uence of gender and old age stereotypes on sexual behaviour in late adulthood. Interestingly, the impact of gender stereotypes can only be noticed in a group of women. The infl uence of old age stereotypes is restricted to modifying the level of sexual techniques variety. It is signifi cant that large sex-related differences are observed in the strength of the stereotype impact on sexual behaviours under study.
Maria Kirejczyk
Developmental Psychology, Volume 14, Issue 4, 2009, pp. 57-72
Effective functioning of the family system depends on its ability to adjust to inevitable changes associated with the development of all the members. Patterns of familial communication develop as a child grows up. Adolescence seems to be a period, when a change in the attitude of parents toward their child is required; parents should begin treating the child as an adult. The purpose of the present study was to identify differences in patterns of parents-child interaction at the beginning and at the end of adolescence. Two groups of families were investigated: with an adolescent child (lower secondary school students (“gimnazjum”)) and with an adult child (university students). Experimental method was used to assess patterns of communication and its effi ciency. Factor analysis was employed to reveal basic patterns of interaction. They were associated with the leading role of one of the members. Four different patterns were identifi ed: „child-centred”, „mother-centred”, „father-centred” and without a leader. The patterns occurred irrespective of a family having an adolescent or an adult child. Surprisingly, it was parental education which turned out to be associated with communication pattern. Mothers in „mother-centred” families were signifi cantly better educated than other mothers. Communication in these families was the most effi cient. „Child-centred” pattern was linked with the father’s poor education. Communication in these families was less effi cient. In the present study, patterns of familial communication did not correlate with a child’s age. Perhaps these patterns are not as fl exible as expected. The main conclusion of this study is that probably other factors influence patterns of parents-child interaction.
Elżbieta Talik
Developmental Psychology, Volume 14, Issue 4, 2009, pp. 73-86
Adolescents experience many stressful situations, concerning mainly personal problems as well as family, school, and peers problems. In such stressful situations, they employ different coping strategies. The aim of this study was to determine the types of coping strategies depending on the character of a stressful situation. 451 adolescents were investigated (278 girls and 173 boys) with Ways of Coping Questionnaire by R. Lazarus and S. Folkman, prepared by Z. Uchnast (1995), Religious Coping Questionnaire (RCOPE) by K. I. Pargament in Polish adaptation by E. Talik and L. Szewczyk (2008) and an original Survey. Results of the research demonstrate that in addition to traditional coping strategies, adolescents use religious coping strategies as well.
Ewa Gurba, Anna Kołodziejczyk, Małgorzata Stępień-Nycz
Developmental Psychology, Volume 14, Issue 4, 2009, pp. 89-96
Anna Rybka
Developmental Psychology, Volume 14, Issue 4, 2009, pp. 97-103
Publication date: 2009
Issue Editor: Michał Grygielski
Arkadiusz Białek
Developmental Psychology, Volume 14, Issue 3, 2009, pp. 9-21
The paper investigates the presence of perspective-induction and implicit form of perspective-taking (perspective-comply) in a triadic joint engagement and treats them as foundational for explicit perspective-taking. The analysis focuses on the participation of those processes in such behaviours as gaze-following, social referencing, pointing, and joint action. By engagement in these activities children provide the evidence of implicit understanding of separateness of one’s own perspective and that of the others. Due to their reference to the psychological common ground emerging between them, participants of a coordinated joint engagement are able to adequately understand their activities. Development of the ability for explicit (articulated) perspective-taking is possible only as an outcome of participations in joint attention and joint engagement.
Anna Kołodziejczyk
Developmental Psychology, Volume 14, Issue 3, 2009, pp. 23-37
The paper presents the benefits that can be derived from studying consumer socialisation in the light of the development of child’s mental competences. In the beginning, the author presents how consumer socialization, the process by which young people acquire skills, knowledge and attitudes relevant to their functioning as consumers in the market place, is conceived in four different theories of: James McNeal (1992, 2007), Patti Valkenburg, Joanne Cantor (2001), Fiony Cram, Sik Hung Ng (1999), Deborah Roedder John (1999). The analysis of presented theoretical approaches show that they all emphasize the role of socio-cognitive development as developmental mechanisms crucial for the child’s improved consumer functioning, but do not explain which developmental competences are the most crucial. While the theories draw heavily on Piagetan developmental theory, the author argues that the more recent approach focusing on the development of the child’s theories of mind may prove more fruitful. The developmental changes in two areas: children’s ability to appreciate and cope with advertising and children’s influence in purchase decision can be better explained by growing mental competences of the child. The review of research on these topics generates the hypothesis that first- as well second-order mental states understanding is important for generating and understanding persuasive communication. Future studies, based on this hypothesis, can be valuable for understanding the developmental mechanisms of consumer socialisation as well as a theory of mind development in middle childhood.
Weronika Puchała
Developmental Psychology, Volume 14, Issue 3, 2009, pp. 41-51
The aim of this research was to analyse the relationship between adolescents’ self-knowledge (general self-esteem level, self-esteem coherence, self-esteem certainty) and locus of control, on the one hand, and action control level (decision-related action control level – AOD and action control level during performing an activity – AOP) on the other.
119 eighteen-year-old students of two Warsaw secondary schools participated in the study. The methods used to analyse the hypotheses were: 1) M. Tukałło and R. Wiechnik’s Self-Knowledge Inventory, 2) R. Drwal’s Locus of Control Questionnaire “Delta” and 3) J. Kuhl’s Action Control Scale. SPSS pack (Pearson’s ‘r’ correlation method, regression method, Pearson’s partial correlation method) was used for the purpose of statistical analysis.
The results of the research point to a statistically signifi cant correlation between self-esteem and decision-related action control as well as between locus of control and decision-related action control. Moreover, the findings demonstrate that locus of control and general self-esteem are not correlated with action control during performing an activity. However, the study confirmed a significant, but not very strong, correlation between action control during performing an activity and global self-esteem, intellectual self-esteem and self-esteem certainty.
Anna Kraśkiewicz, Ewa Czerniawska
Developmental Psychology, Volume 14, Issue 3, 2009, pp. 53-64
The purpose of the experiment was to examine the effect of atmospheric music on time estimations in a group of teenagers. One hundred and fifty students of a Warsaw secondary school took part in the experiment. It turned out that the number of tunes presented to students influenced their time perception. The estimated duration of a given interval was longer when many as opposed to a few songs were played. Moreover, the tempo of background music modified the effect of number of songs on time perception. When fast songs were played number of tunes influenced time estimations more as opposed to the situation when slow music was presented. Other variables had little or no effect on the respondents’ time perception: gender, perceived tempo of music, liking/disliking of songs and the level of disturbance in performing a cognitive task. In the experiment students made retrospective duration judgment. They answered the temporal questions verbally.
Grażyna Katra
Developmental Psychology, Volume 14, Issue 3, 2009, pp. 65-76
Future time perspective (FTP) is one of the most habitual manifestations of prospect action. It plays an important role in establishing life plans and projects. An individual with a vast time perspective can more easily articulate far-reaching goals as well as obtain resources to achieve them. Additionally, undertaken present actions often have immediate as well as long-term effects and the ability to anticipate them is important. Both aspects are necessary for life success.
The goal of this study was to validate the two following hypotheses. The first stipulates that a developed future time perspective (PPC) is related to the ability to consider future consequences (CFC). The second hypothesis states that the development of formal operatory thought favours the formation of both aforementioned characteristics. A total of 127 students from 5 fi rst-year secondary school classes participated in the survey in which each completed a FTP test and the CFC Scale. The presented CFC Scale was adapted to serve the purpose of this study. The results verify that PPC and CFC are strongly correlated (0.73). Furthermore, the level of formal thinking (measured by the Test of Formal Operations) has a signifi cant meaning for FTP (F(2, 124) = 52.73; p<0.001) as well as for the trends of behaviour assessment of future consequences (F(2, 124) = 47.61; p<0.001).
Urszula Dębska, Halina Guła-Kubiszewska, Wojciech Starościak, Iwona Bielawska
Developmental Psychology, Volume 14, Issue 3, 2009, pp. 77-88
Numerous transformations occurring in contemporary world concern also health and related behaviour. Increasing adaptive and preventive activities of individuals are becoming essential. New educational curricula introduce issues related to health, behaviour and healthy life styles.
The research problem presented in the article concerns health behaviour of people in early (390 respondents) and late adulthood (117 respondents). The respondents were students of the Academy of Physical Education or pedagogy, and a fraction of elderly people consisted of students of the University of the Third Age (numbering 63). Our research aimed to compare prohealth activity among the respondents in order to answer the following questions: what the extent of their health activity is and whether health education is refl ected in behaviour. G. Dolińska’s Health Behaviour Survey and Inventory was employed. The research conducted confirmed the existence of significant differences among the respondents. The differences include both the intensity of behaviour and a predominance of certain types. The respondents exhibit an average level of health behaviour, although its increased intensity has been observed in late adulthood, especially among the students of the University of the Third Age. Also the students of health education yielded higher results than the remaining early adults.
Paulina Mrozińska
Developmental Psychology, Volume 14, Issue 3, 2009, pp. 89-100
The main purpose of the research is the analysis of the relationship between the experience of spouses’ loneliness and preferred strategies of coping with loneliness. The research is based on Ami Rokach’s classification of strategies which people use in the process of coping with loneliness. She proposes the following strategies: Self-Development and Understanding, Reflection and Acceptance, Distancing and Denial, Religion and Faith, Social Support Network, Increased Activity. Sixty married couples were examined. Respondents’ age ranged between 22 and 67. The youngest couple has been married for 3 months, the eldest – for 43 years. A dependent variable is the preferred strategy of coping with loneliness. The main independent variable is the experience of loneliness, while other independent variables were controlled: spouses’ gender, age and marriage duration. Rokach’s questionnaire (The Experience of Loneliness-coping with Loneliness) was used in order to examine the preferred strategies. UCLA Loneliness Scale-Version 3 was used in order to examine the experience of loneliness. Data gathered indicates that spouses feel lonely because of the lack of intimate relationships. The experience of loneliness depends on spouses’ age and marriage duration: the older spouses are, the lonelier they feel. The most preferred strategy is Building Social Bridges. The choice of strategies depends on spouses’ gender, age, marriage duration, and the experience of loneliness.
Anna Cieślik, Kamila Madeja-Bień
Developmental Psychology, Volume 14, Issue 3, 2009, pp. 103-107
Małgorzata Stępień-Nycz
Developmental Psychology, Volume 14, Issue 3, 2009, pp. 109-113
Publication date: 2009
Issue editor: Maria Kielar-Turska
Stefan Frydrychowicz
Developmental Psychology, Volume 14, Issue 2, 2009, pp. 13-28
The article attempts to classify views on communicative competence. It presents a hypothesis concerning its origins and development. The development of communicative competence is analyzed in the context of differentiation and consolidation of various forms of changeability of communicational situation. The article successively deals with the following issues:
1. Competence. Linguistic competence and communicative competence.
2. Viewpoints on communicative competence.
3. Origins and development of communicative competence.
The conclusion touches upon the influence of effective interpersonal communication on the development of a human being.
Małgorzata Stępień-Nycz
Developmental Psychology, Volume 14, Issue 2, 2009, pp. 29-39
The term “emotional discourse” refers to the child’s participation in conversations about emotions. A great body of research (Dunn et al., 1991; Dunn, Brophy, 2005; Harris, 2005; Peterson, Slaughter, 2006) indicate that there is a significant correlation between the child’s participation in emotional discourse (with his/her mother, siblings or friends) and his/her emotional knowledge. The aim of the article is to analyze the role of emotional discourse in the development of the knowledge of emotions.
Theories of discourse (van Dijk, 1985; 1990; Shugar, 1995) and of cognitive development (Karmiloff-Smith, 1992; Stemplewska-Żakowicz, 2001; Tomasello, 2002) indicate that in discourse the child has an opportunity to gain knowledge about invisible phenomena and is confronted with distinct perspectives. The child has also an opportunity to name and organize his/her emotions and to make distinctions between them due to verbal terms provided by his/her partner in discourse. Therefore the child’s participation in emotional discourse facilitaties his/her becom ing aware of his/her emotional states together with their causes and consequences, and – due to confrontation with distinct perspectives – the development of the discursive representation of emotions.
Stanisław Milewski
Developmental Psychology, Volume 14, Issue 2, 2009, pp. 43-56
Studies devoted to phonostatic research have made clear that there is a correlation between the frequency of words of a certain phoneme and syllable length and their adherence to certain stylistic variations of Polish. The fact of the occurrence of a higher number of shorter or longer words depending on the type of analysed expressions was noticed by linguists at the beginning of the 20th century, although most of these opinions were not yet supported by a proper quantity of data. Such data began to published in the 1970s.
My article presents results concerning the frequency of n-phoneme and n-syllable words in a particular, simplified register of infant-directed speech (IDS). A comparison of the frequency of the occurrence of words of a certain phoneme and syllable length in IDS with the data obtained from other speech samples, among them the Polish of pre-schoolers and contemporary Polish, allows one to state that the length of words may be one of the most important criteria evidencing the “difference” of infant-directed speech.
The analysed language material (cassette recording) came from 102 child-carers (mothers, fathers and grandmothers). It was listened to, phonologically transcribed and entered into a computer memory by one person. All of the analysed text consisted of 74,534 words (308,918 phonemes).
Ewa Haman, Krzysztof Fronczyk, Iwona Girtler
Developmental Psychology, Volume 14, Issue 2, 2009, pp. 57-71
The paper presents a study on lexical development of Polish children aged 2–6. Three tools were constructed for this purpose: “The Child Lexicon Development Questionnaire/Checklist”, “The Questionnaire: Communication and Family Routines“ and a personal questionnaire. All were filled in by children’s parents. The first tool comprised nouns, verbs and adjectives drawn in a layered drawing from the Frequency List of Polish Language Corpus (PWN, 2004). The next two consisted of questions related to interactions in the family, social environment and family SES. Data from 148 children were analyzed. Results revealed non-linear lexical growth with age, positive impact of diversified interactions with parents as well as of a number of different games/play situations with peers on children’s vocabulary. Interactions with parents mainly affected noun and adjective vocabulary, while the number of plays with peers affected verb vocabulary.
Ewa Czaplewska
Developmental Psychology, Volume 14, Issue 2, 2009, pp. 73-82
Communication can be both verbal and non-verbal. Often a person’s true intentions are expressed non-verbally. The two main factors of non-verbal expression are voice and facial expression.
Research has shown (Edwards, Manstead and MacDonalds, 1984; Głodowski, 1999) that children who decipher non-verbal signals most adeptly are the most popular among their peers, while those who show a lack of ability in this matter may experience social and educational failures.
The purpose of this study was to determine: 1. If and to what extent preschool-aged children (4;0–7;11 years) recognize facial signs of emotion. 2. If and to what extent preschool-aged children recognize vocal signs of emotion. 3. If, in the case of a contradiction between the verbal and non-verbal message, pre-school children emphasize the content or the intonation of speech.
This research involved the participation of 113 children attending pre-schools in the Tri- City. Analysis of the study results allowed us to state that as children mature, one can observe an increase in their ability to decipher emotions from facial expressions, while their ability to recognize emotions expressed by voice modulation decreases. Moreover, it appears that in the case of ambiguous messages, younger children place more emphasis on voice intonation than their older peers, whereas older children prefer the message content as the main indicator of information.
Anna Szczypczyk
Developmental Psychology, Volume 14, Issue 2, 2009, pp. 83-93
Research studies on ambiguity are related to different areas – communication, language and processing of perceptual stimuli. Results obtained from most research show that children under the age of six can neither react dequately to ambiguous linguistic or visual stimuli nor understand ambiguous statements.
A hypothesis about the relation between understanding ambiguity and the development of the knowledge about the mind was proposed after an analysis of existing experimental data. Four different tasks testing the understanding of ambiguity had been used and it was found out that their level of difficulty varied. There was no connection between the performance in solving tasks of different types. The relation between the understanding of ambiguity and the development of the knowledge of mind was indicated. Children performed better in false beliefs test than in tasks testing their understanding of ambiguity. There was a correlation between performance in particular tasks testing the understanding of false beliefs and tasks testing the understanding of ambiguity.
Ewa Dryll
Developmental Psychology, Volume 14, Issue 2, 2009, pp. 95-109
The aim of the presented empirical study was to explore the development of metaphor understanding in children. The results of the study were to reveal the way the mind organises information belonging to different fields of knowledge. The study followed a quasi-experimental design. The subjects were 77 children from three age groups: 5;6–6;0, 8;0–8;6, 9;6–10;0. The dependent variable was the level of comprehension of metaphors with vehicles from the animal domain, but connoting human attributes. The variable was measured through individual Piagetian interviews. The study proper was preceded by a pilot study on 24 adults. The data collected were analysed with the help of raters. The study confirmed the hypothesis that the ability to activate metaphorical thinking increases with age, however the increase is not linear. Older children assigned more human dispositional traits, thoughts and preferences to the objects of metaphors. Younger ones often focused on the physical features of animals. With age, the tendency to give positive evaluations to the objects of metaphors increases, while the number of digressions decreases. The development of the understanding of conventional metaphors may take a different course than the development of the understanding of creative ones. The most distinct changes in the ability of understanding metaphors concerning humans appear at the age of eight.
Anna Świątek
Developmental Psychology, Volume 14, Issue 2, 2009, pp. 113-116
Małgorzata Stępień-Nycz
Developmental Psychology, Volume 14, Issue 2, 2009, pp. 117-122
Publication date: 2009
Issue editor: Jan Łuczyński
Piotr Oleś, Monika Kłosok-Ścibich
Developmental Psychology, Volume 14, Issue 1, 2009, pp. 9-25
Gauguin syndrome is a phenomenon described by life span psychology, referring to the intention to stimulate the changes of personality by means of a change of life circumstances. The phenomenon is patterned on Paul Gauguin's life, fuli of sudden and unexpected turns. Gauguin syndrome is an example of the change of life linę during the so called midlife crisis and it implies personality change: an attempt to discover one's own identity and intention to construct a new identity and self-concept. In this article we propose a few criteria defining Gauguin syndrome: (1) important change of life priorities, (2) undertaking a new form of activity, (3) which justi-fies meaning of life, (4) is based on different values than before, (5) gives a sense of freedom and authenticity, (6) decision about such a change is undertaken by a person independently, without any support from other people, and (7) it implies the decline of social and economic status of a person. In this article we also emphasize the differentiation of the Gauguin myth and syndrome, with possible explanations of the phenomena. Three case studies arę presented including life histories according to McAdams approach. The empirical data suggests that an important change of life might be announced by previous experiences.
Dorota Turska
Developmental Psychology, Volume 14, Issue 1, 2009, pp. 27-34
The personological trend inpsychology of creativity has underlined the exceptionality of the creator and the creative process. This point of view forms social attitudes and convictions rela-ted to criteria of selection of human behavior and works worth recording. Opinions appear in literaturę that the aforementioned convictions take on the form of myths about creativity.
Mythical comdctions about creativity may be the essence of so-called hidden theories of creativity, which arę employed not only by laymen but also by professionals - like teachers. I intend to explain in this manner the difference between teacher's declarations about their affirmation ofpupiPs creativity and their activity in practice.
The article contains a review of current educational research, and its effects on the influence of myths concerning creativity and their negative implications on education.
Agnieszka Szymańska
Developmental Psychology, Volume 14, Issue 1, 2009, pp. 37-47
The differences in parental control distribution were explored (which was conceptualized as teaching children's rules) and the demand of immediate obedience. PAiNK scalę was used to assess the level of parental control and discipline. The sample consisted of two groups of preschool children's parents. Children were 4 to 6 years old. The whole sample consisted of 204 people, 51 mothers and 51 fathers of „difficult" children and 51 fathers and 51 mothers of well-behaved children. The research was carried out in Poland's three big cities: Warsaw, Cracow and Częstochowa.
Hypotheses were presented that parents of "difficult" children would achieve higher scores in demanding immediate obedience, parents of well-behaved children would achieve higher scores in parental control. Two way multivariate Analysis of Yariance (MANOYA) confirmed research hypotheses: the distribution of parental control differs in both groups. Ań interaction effect between parental role and upbringing difficulty for obedience demanding yariable was discovered. Outcomes demonstrate crucial role of explanations and time in assimilating rules.
Beata Winnicka
Developmental Psychology, Volume 14, Issue 1, 2009, pp. 49-59
According to the models emphasizing the role of memory in estimating time the morę elements the phenomenon contains, the longer is its subjective duration. Research on space perception shows that the subjective path length is positively related to the number of landmarks associated with a given route. The aim of our experiment was to analyze how the density of space influences the judgments of time needed to traverse it in children aged 4, 6 and 9 years.
120 children (20 girls an 20 boy s in each agę group) were tested individually. We presented them two 15-second videos that differ in the number of presented objects. Participants produ-ced prospective time estimations, using the specially designed clock. 9-year olds perceived space with a greater density with longer durations F(1.39) = 11.50, p < 0.005. The tendency for lengthening subjective time as aresult of increased density of space has appeared also in preschool groups, but the differences in the 4- and the 6-year-olds were not statistically significant.
Kinga Dziwańska
Developmental Psychology, Volume 14, Issue 1, 2009, pp. 61-70
The presented research is based on the human activities theories (Nuttin's concept of self behaviour (1980), Tomaszewski's theory of activities (1975) and Miller's, Galanter's and Pribram's theory of plans (1980)) as well as on the future time perspective of human beings theory (Nuttin 1980, Nurmi 1991, Zaleski 1991).
The corę problem of the research focuses on the defmition of specificity of establishing short term plans (not longer than half year) by youth in relation to its agę.
The author uses her ąuestionnaire "Future planning" constructed for the purpose of the research, which was created especially for studies on process of planning. The survey was conducted in 2004 in Warsaw and involved population a sample of 300 people (15-year-old students of grammar schools and 18-year-old students of secondary schools).
The analysis of research results allows to draw up the following conclusions. Process of establishing plans by youth has its own specificity and is dependent on the youth agę. In comparison to students of grammar schools students of secondary schools use advanced planning strategies morę often (they gather information, mąkę notes, use diaries). The statistical analysis of the research results allows to conclude that students of grammar schools take advantage of generał strategies, while students of secondary schools - of detailed strategies in the process of establishing plans.
Agnieszka Bieńkowska
Developmental Psychology, Volume 14, Issue 1, 2009, pp. 71-80
The aim of the research was to find relationship between anxiety-trait, emotional reactivity and superstitiousness. There were 75 six-class pupils chosen as research participants (33 girls and 42 boys) recruited from one of primary schools near Warsaw. Statistical analysis show statistically significant positive correlation between anxiety-trait and superstitiousness (r = 0,33;p<0,01)andnearly statistically significant positive correlation between emotional reactivity and superstitions (r = 0,22; p < 0,06). In result of the research it also turned out that as far as superstitions arę concerned, there arę gender differences: girls arę morę superstitious thanboys (F = 23,81 (1; 73); p < 0,001).
Joanna Kossewska
Developmental Psychology, Volume 14, Issue 1, 2009, pp. 83-88
Kamil Jezierski
Developmental Psychology, Volume 14, Issue 1, 2009, pp. 89-92