Zimne versus gorące funkcje zarządzające i język a rozumienie przez dzieci własnych oraz innych osób stanów umysłowych
cytuj
pobierz pliki
RIS BIB ENDNOTEWybierz format
RIS BIB ENDNOTEZimne versus gorące funkcje zarządzające i język a rozumienie przez dzieci własnych oraz innych osób stanów umysłowych
Data publikacji: 06.03.2014
Psychologia Rozwojowa, 2013, Tom 18, Numer 4, s. 65 - 82
Autorzy
Zimne versus gorące funkcje zarządzające i język a rozumienie przez dzieci własnych oraz innych osób stanów umysłowych
Cool vs hot executive functions, language and children’s understanding of their own vs others’ mental states
The main purpose of this study has been to verify a hypothesis based on the assumptions of the theories by Russell (1998) and Zelazo et al. (2005), concerning the relation between the development of executive functions (EF) and theory of mind (ToM). It was hypothesized that the level of the hot EF compared to the cool EF would be more strongly connected with children’s understanding of their own mental states than those of others. In the analysis of relationships between selected aspects of the EF and ToM, the level of language development was also considered. The study covered forty-four 3- and 4-year-olds, using false-belief tasks in their “unexpected contents” version (Gopnik & Astington, 1988) as the measures of ToM development, with one question requiring the attribution of a false belief to somebody else, and another question requiring the recognition of one’s own previous belief as false. The cool EF were measured with a version of the Bear-Dragon task (Reed et al., 1984), and the hot EF with the Children’s Gambling Task (Kerr, Zelazo, 2004). Language development was assessed with the Picture Vocabulary Test (OTS-R; Haman et al., 2011). It appeared that children’s understanding of their own mental states in both age groups was related only to the cool EF, while their understanding of other people’s mental states correlated marginally with the cool EF only in 4-year-olds. The level of the hot EF was not connected signifi cantly with any aspect of ToM. Regression analysis revealed that the cool aspect of the EF was a signifi cant language-independent predictor of the understanding of one’s own mental states in 4-year-olds, but not in 3-year-olds. The results are at variance with the predictions based on the theory by Zelazo et al., but consistent with those drawn from Russell’s theory, according to which self-consciousness and self-refl ection underlie the relationship between the EF and ToM.
Astington J.W., Jenkins J.M. (1999), A longitudinal study of the relation between language and theory-of-mind development. Developmental Psychology, 35, 1311–1320.
Bunch K. (2006), A relational complexity approach to the development of hot/cool executive functions. Nieopublikowana praca doktorska. School of Psychology, Griffith University, Gold Coast, Australia.
Carlson S.M., Mandell D.J., Williams L. (2004), Executive function and theory of mind: Stability and prediction from age 2 to 3. Developmental Psychology, 40, 1105–1122.
Carlson S.M., Moses L.J. (2001), Individual differences in inhibitory control and children’s theory of mind. Child Development, 72, 4, 1032–1053.
Frye D., Zelazo P.D., Burack J.A. (1998), I. Cognitive complexity and control: Implications for theory of mind in typical and atypical development. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 7, 116–121.
Frye D., Zelazo P.D., Palfai T. (1995), Theory of mind and rule-based reasoning. Cognitive Development, 10, 483–527.
Gopnik A., Astington J.W. (1988), Children’s understanding of representational change and its relation to the understanding of the false belief and the appearance-reality distinction. Child Development, 59, 26–37.
Halford G., Wilson W.H., Phillips S. (1998), Processing capacity defined by relational complexity: Implications for comparative, developmental, and cognitive psychology. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 21, 803–864.
Haman E., Fronczyk K., Miękisz O. (2011), Ocena zasobu słownictwa u dzieci w wieku przedszkolnym – nowe narzędzie testowe. Psychologia Rozwojowa, 15, 1, 21–45.
Hogrefe G.J., Wimmer H., Perner J. (1986), Ignorance versus false belief: A developmental lag in attribution of epistemic states. Child Development, 57, 567–582.
Hongwanishkul D., Happaney K.R., Lee W.S.C., Zelazo P.D. (2005), Assessment of hot and cool executive function in young children: Age-related changes and individual differences. Developmental Neuropsychology, 28, 2, 617–644.
Hughes C., Ensor R. (2005), Executive function and theory of mind: A family affair? Developmental Neuropsychology, 28, 2, 645–668.
Hughes C., Ensor R. (2007), Executive function and theory of mind: Predictive relations from ages 2 to 4. Developmental Psychology, 43, 1447–1459.
Hughes C., Ensor R. (2009), How do families help or hinder the emergence of early executive function? [w:] C. Lewis, J.M. Carpendale (eds.), Social interaction and the development of executive function. New Directions in Child and Adolescent Development, 123, 35–50.
Jacques S., Zelazo P.D. (2005), Language and the development of cognitive flexibility: Implications for theory of mind [w:] J.W. Astington, J.A. Baird (eds.), Why language matters for theory of mind, 144–162. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Kerr A., Zelazo P.D. (2004), Development of “hot” executive function: The Children’s Gambling Task. Brain and Cognition, 55, 148–157.
Liss M., Fein D., Allen D., Dunn M., Feinstein C., Morris R. i in. (2001), Executive functioning in high-functioning children with autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42, 261–270.
Luria A.R. (1961), The role of speech in the regulation of normal and abnormal behaviour. New York: Per- gamon Press.
Milligan K., Astington J.W., Dack L.A. (2007), Language and theory of mind: Meta-analysis of the relation between language ability and false-belief understanding. Child Development, 78, 2, 622–646.
Mitchell R.W., Neal M. (2005a), Children’s understanding of their own and others’ mental states. Part A. Self- understanding precedes understanding of others in pretence. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 23, 175–200.
Mitchell R.W., Neal M. (2005b), Children’s understanding of their own and others’ mental states. Part B. Understanding of others precedes self-understanding for some false beliefs. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 23, 201–208.
Moses L.J. (2001), Executive accounts of theory-of-mind development. Child Development, 72, 688–690.
Moses L.J., Carlson S.M., Sabbagh M.A. (2005), On the specifity of the relation between executive function and children’s theories of mind [w:] W. Schneider, R. Schumann-Hengsteler, B. Sodian (eds.), Young children’s cognitive development. Interrelationships among executive functioning, working memory, verbal ability, and theory of mind, 131–146. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Perner J., Lang B., Kloo D. (2002), Theory of mind and self-control: More than a common problem of inhibition. Child Development, 73, 752–767.
Perner J., Leekam S.R., Wimmer H. (1987), Three-year olds’ difficulty with false belief: The case for a conceptual deficit. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 5, 125–137.
Putko A. (2008), Dziecięca „teoria umysłu” w fazie jawnej i utajonej a funkcje wykonawcze. Poznań: Wydawnictwo Naukowe UAM.
Putko A. (2011), Teoria umysłu a zimne versus gorące funkcje zarządzające u dzieci w wieku przedszkolnym. Psychologia Rozwojowa, 16, 1, 73–84.
Reed M., Pien D.L., Rothbart M.K. (1984). Inhibitory self-control in preschool children. Merrill Palmer Quarterly, 30, 131–147.
Russell J. (1998), How executive disorders can bring about an inadequate ‘theory of mind’ [w:] J. Russell (ed.), Autism as an executive disorder, 256–299. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Russell J. (2002), Cognitive theories of autism [w:] J.E. Harrison, A.M. Owen (eds.), Cognitive deficits and brain disorders, 295–324. London: Dunitz Martin.
Russell J., Jarrold C., Hood B. (1999), Two intact executive capacities in children with autism: Implications for the core executive dysfunctions in the disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 29, 103–112.
Russell J., Mauthner N., Sharpe S., Tidswell T. (1991), The ‘windows task’ as a measure of strategic deception in preschoolers and autistics subjects. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 9, 331–349.
Wellman H.M., Cross D., Watson J. (2001), Meta-analysis of theory-of-mind development: The truth about false belief. Child Development, 72, 655–684.
Welsh M.C., Friedman S.L., Spieker S.J. (2006), Executive functions in developing children: Current conceptualizations and questions for the future [w:] K. McCartney, D. Phillips (eds.), Blackwell handbook of early childhood development, 167–187. Oxford: Blackwell.
Zelazo P.D., Müller U. (2002), Executive functions in typical and atypical development [w:] U. Goswami (ed.), Handbook of childhood cognitive development, 445–469. Oxford: Blackwell.
Zelazo P.D., Qu L., Müller U. (2005), Hot and cool aspects of executive function: Relations in early development [w:] W. Schneider, R. Schumann-Hengsteler, B. Sodian (eds.), Young children’s cognitive development Interrelationships among executive functioning, working memory, verbal ability, and theory of mind, 71–93. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Informacje: Psychologia Rozwojowa, 2013, Tom 18, Numer 4, s. 65 - 82
Typ artykułu: Oryginalny artykuł naukowy
Tytuły:
Zimne versus gorące funkcje zarządzające i język a rozumienie przez dzieci własnych oraz innych osób stanów umysłowych
Cool vs hot executive functions, language and children’s understanding of their own vs others’ mental states
Uniwersytet Adama Mickiewicza w Poznaniu, Wieniawskiego 1, 61-712 Poznań
Publikacja: 06.03.2014
Status artykułu: Otwarte
Licencja: Żadna
Udział procentowy autorów:
Korekty artykułu:
-Języki publikacji:
PolskiLiczba wyświetleń: 3157
Liczba pobrań: 2001