FAQ
logotyp Uniwersytetu Jagiellońskiego

Ekspozycja na fabularną prozę a spontaniczna versus jawna mentalizacja

Data publikacji: 31.03.2020

Psychologia Rozwojowa, 2020, Tom 25, Numer 1, s. 49 - 67

https://doi.org/10.4467/20843879PR.20.003.12000

Autorzy

,
Adam Putko
Uniwersytet Adama Mickiewicza w Poznaniu, Wieniawskiego 1, 61-712 Poznań
https://orcid.org/0000-0001-7770-0046 Orcid
Wszystkie publikacje autora →
,
Agnieszka Michałowska
Uniwersytet Adama Mickiewicza w Poznaniu, Wieniawskiego 1, 61-712 Poznań
Wszystkie publikacje autora →
Agata Złotogórska-Suwińska
Uniwersytet Adama Mickiewicza w Poznaniu, Wieniawskiego 1, 61-712 Poznań
https://orcid.org/0000-0002-5075-2900 Orcid
Wszystkie publikacje autora →

Tytuły

Ekspozycja na fabularną prozę a spontaniczna versus jawna mentalizacja

Abstrakt

Exposure to Narrative Fiction and Spontaneous versus Explicit Mentalization

The main objective of the study was to check for the existence of the effect of short-term exposure to fiction in the area of mentalization abilities (Kidd, Castano, 2013), taking into account the spontaneous versus explicit aspect of these abilities. An additional aim was to verify the hypothesis that at the root of this phenomenon lies the process of imaginary simulation. In the first part of the study, participants (N = 103, aged 18 to 25) were randomly assigned to one of four groups: (1) reading literary fiction, (2) listening to the same story as the first group, and at the same time imagining the events presented in it, (3) reading popular fiction, and (4) not reading any story. In the second part of the study, all participants completed mentalization tests, a test checking familiarity with fiction, and the questionnaire on practices of reading books, magazines and watching television. The study showed no significant differences between the groups in the results of mentalization tests, even under conditions conducive to imaginary simulation. Familiarity with fiction proved to be a positive predictor of spontaneous mentalization, and exposure to television was a negative predictor of explicit mentalization. The findings lead to the conclusion that short-term exposure to fiction does not lead to a significant increase in spontaneous or explicit mentalization. However, both aspects of mentalization are connected with more long-term factors that are expressed in familiarity with fiction and exposure to television.

Bibliografia

Acheson D.J., Wells J.B., MacDonald M.C. (2008), New and Updated Tests of Print Exposure and Reading Abilities in College Students. Behavior Research Methods, 40, 278–289.

Baron-Cohen S. (1995), Mindblindness: An essay on Autism and Theory of Mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Baron-Cohen S., Wheelwright S., Jolliffe T. (1997), Is there a “Language of the Eyes”? Evidence from Normal Adults and Adults with Autism or Asperger Syndrome. Visual Cognition, 4, 311–331.

Baron-Cohen S., Wheelwright S., Hill J., Raste Y., Plumb I. (2001), The „Reading the Mind in the Eyes” Test Revised Version: A Study with Normal Adults, and Adults with Asperger Syndrome or High-Functioning Autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42, 241–251.

Barthes R. (1999 [1970]), S/Z, tłum. M.P. Markowski, M. Gołębiewska. Warszawa: Wydawnictwo KR.

Black J.E., Barnes J.L. (2015), The Effects of Reading Material on Social and Non-Social Cognition. Poetics, 52, 32–43.

Clements W.A., Perner J. (1994), Implicit Understanding of Belief. Cognitive Development, 9, 377–395.

Converse B.A., Lin S., Keysar B., Epley N. (2008), In the Mood to Get Over Yourself: Mood Affects Theoryof- Mind Use. Emotion, 8, 725–730.

DeLillo D. (2013 [2011]), Anioł Esmeralda. Dziewięć opowiadań, tłum. K. Obłucki. Warszawa: Oficyna Literacka Noir sur Blanc .

Dijkstra N., Bosch S.E., Gerven M.A.J. van (2019), Shared Neural Mechanisms of Visual Perception and Imagery. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 23(5), 423–434.

Dodell-Feder D., Lincoln S.H., Coulson J.P., Hooker C.I. (2013), Using Fiction to Assess Mental State Understanding: A New Task for Assessing Theory of Mind in Adults. PLoS ONE, 8(11).

Forster E.M. (1927), Aspects of the Novel. London: Edward Arnold & Company.

Green M.C., Brock T.C. (2000), The Role of Transportation in the Persuasiveness of Public Narratives. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79(5), 701–721.

Green M.C., Brock T.C. (2002), In the Mind’s Eye: Transportation-Imagery Model of Narrative Persuasion. W: M.C. Green, J.J. Strange, T.C. Brock (red.), Narrative Impact: Social Cognitive Foundations, 315–341.  Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Happé F. (1994), An Advanced Test of Theory of Mind: Understanding of Story Characters’ Thoughts and Feelings by Able Autistic, Mentally Handicapped, and Normal Children and Adults. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24, 129–154.

Harris P.L. (2005), Conversation, Pretense, and Theory of Mind. W: J.W. Astington, J.A. Baird (red.), Why Language Matters for Theory of Mind, 70–83. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Hemingway E. (1963 [1930]), Rzeka dwóch serc i inne opowiadania, tłum. B. Zieliński. Warszawa: Państwowy Instytut Wydawniczy.

Kidd D.C., Castano E. (2013), Reading Literary Fiction Improves Theory of Mind. Science, 342, 377–380.

Kidd D.C., Castano E. (2017a), Different Stories: How Levels of Familiarity with Literary and Genre Fiction Relate to Mentalizing. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 11(4), 474–486.

Kidd D.C., Castano E. (2017b), Panero et al. (2016): Failure to Replicate Methods Caused the Failure to Replicate Results. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 112(3), e1–e4.

Kidd D.C., Ongis M., Castano E. (2016), On Literary Fiction and Its Effects on Theory of Mind. Scientific Study of Literature, 6(1), 42–58.

Levin J.R., Divine-Hawkins P. (1974), Visual Imagery as a Prose-Learning Process. Journal of Reading Behavior, 6(1), 23–30.

Mar R.A., Oatley K. (2008), The Function of Fiction is the Abstraction and Simulation of Social Experience. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 3, 173–192.

Mar R.A., Tackett J.L., Moore C. (2010), Exposure to Media and Theory-of-Mind Development in Preschoolers. Cognitive Development, 25, 69–78.

Nathanson A.I., Sharp M.L., Aladé F., Rasmussen E.E., Christy K. (2013), The Relation between Television Exposure and Theory of Mind Among Preschoolers. Journal of Communication, 63, 1088–1108.

Nowicki S. (2010), Manual for the Receptive Tests of the Diagnostic Analysis of Nonverbal Accuracy 2. Atlanta, GA: Emory University, Department of Psychology.

Oatley K. (2016), Fiction: Simulation of Social Worlds. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 20(8), 618–628.

Paivio A. (1971), Imagery and Verbal Processes. New York: Holt.

Panero M.E., Weisberg D.S., Black J., Goldstein T.R., Barnes J.L., Brownell H., Winner E. (2016), Does Reading a Single Passage of Literary Fiction Really Improve Theory of Mind? An Attempt at Replication. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 111(3), 46–54.

Panero M.E., Weisberg D.S., Black J., Goldstein T.R., Barnes J.L., Brownell H., Winner E. (2017), No Support for the Claim That Literary Fiction Uniquely and Immediately Improves Theory of Mind: A Reply to Kidd and Castano’s Commentary on Panero et al. (2016). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 112(3), e5–e8.

Perner J. (1991), Understanding the Representational Mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Pino M.C., Mazza M. (2016), The Use of „Literary Fiction” to Promote Mentalizing Ability. PLoS ONE, 11(8).

Rynkiewicz A., Ptaszek K. (2013), „Odczytywanie stanów umysłu na podstawie oczu” w wersji dla dorosłych. Gdańsk: Centrum Diagnozy, Terapii i Edukacji Spectrum ASC-Med i Uniwersytet Gdański, Wydział Biologii.

Samur D., Tops M., Koole S.L. (2018), Does a Single Session of Reading Literary Fiction Prime Enhanced Mentalising Performance? Four Replication Experiments of Kidd and Castano (2013). Cognition and Emotion, 32(1), 130–144.

Senju A., Southgate V., White S., Frith U. (2009), Mindblind Eyes: An Absence of Spontaneous Theory of Mind in Asperger Syndrome. Science, 325, 883–885.

Shamay-Tsoory S.G., Aharon-Peretz J. (2007), Dissociable Prefrontal Networks for Cognitive and Affective Theory of Mind: A Lesion Study. Neuropsychologia, 45, 3054–3067.

Stanovich K.E., West R.F. (1989), Exposure to Print and Orthographic Processing. Reading Research Quarterly, 24(4), 402–433.

Steel D. (2012), Grzechy matki, tłum. E. Spirydowicz, M. Kittel. Warszawa: Wydawnictwo Amber.

Tager-Flusberg H., Sullivan K. (2000), A Componential View of Theory of Mind: Evidence from Williams Syndrome. Cognition, 76, 59–89.

Informacje

Informacje: Psychologia Rozwojowa, 2020, Tom 25, Numer 1, s. 49 - 67

Typ artykułu: Oryginalny artykuł naukowy

Tytuły:

Polski:
Ekspozycja na fabularną prozę a spontaniczna versus jawna mentalizacja
Angielski:
Exposure to Narrative Fiction and Spontaneous versus Explicit Mentalization

Autorzy

https://orcid.org/0000-0001-7770-0046

Adam Putko
Uniwersytet Adama Mickiewicza w Poznaniu, Wieniawskiego 1, 61-712 Poznań
https://orcid.org/0000-0001-7770-0046 Orcid
Wszystkie publikacje autora →

Uniwersytet Adama Mickiewicza w Poznaniu, Wieniawskiego 1, 61-712 Poznań

Uniwersytet Adama Mickiewicza w Poznaniu, Wieniawskiego 1, 61-712 Poznań

https://orcid.org/0000-0002-5075-2900

Agata Złotogórska-Suwińska
Uniwersytet Adama Mickiewicza w Poznaniu, Wieniawskiego 1, 61-712 Poznań
https://orcid.org/0000-0002-5075-2900 Orcid
Wszystkie publikacje autora →

Uniwersytet Adama Mickiewicza w Poznaniu, Wieniawskiego 1, 61-712 Poznań

Publikacja: 31.03.2020

Status artykułu: Otwarte __T_UNLOCK

Licencja: CC BY-NC-ND  ikona licencji

Udział procentowy autorów:

Adam Putko (Autor) - 33%
Agnieszka Michałowska (Autor) - 33%
Agata Złotogórska-Suwińska (Autor) - 34%

Korekty artykułu:

-

Języki publikacji:

Polski